13.07.2015 Views

effet du nombre des graphèmes en Anglais - Aix Marseille Université

effet du nombre des graphèmes en Anglais - Aix Marseille Université

effet du nombre des graphèmes en Anglais - Aix Marseille Université

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

App<strong>en</strong>dice II 253DISCUSSIONThe results of this study can be summarized as follows : in a perceptual id<strong>en</strong>tification task, we obtainedlonger id<strong>en</strong>tification times for words having a smaller number of phonemes. This phoneme effect was observedin English and Fr<strong>en</strong>ch for low-frequ<strong>en</strong>cy 5-letter monosyllabic words, but was not obtained in Fr<strong>en</strong>chfor high-frequ<strong>en</strong>cy 5-letter monosyllabic words.The pres<strong>en</strong>t research indicates that grouping letters into graphemes for an effici<strong>en</strong>t orthography-tophonologycomputation requires additional processing time. However, as suggested by curr<strong>en</strong>t computationalmodels of visual word recognition in which word id<strong>en</strong>tification results from two parallel and interdep<strong>en</strong>d<strong>en</strong>tprocesses, whole word orthographic processing and sublexical orthography-to-phonology processing(Coltheart, Curtis, Atkins, & Haller, 1993 ; Jacobs, Rey, Ziegler, & Grainger, 1998), there is a dissociationbetwe<strong>en</strong> the processing of high and low frequ<strong>en</strong>cy words. Indeed, high frequ<strong>en</strong>cy words seem less affected bythe conflicts arising <strong>du</strong>ring the sublexical orthography-to-phonology computation (i.e., the conflict betwe<strong>en</strong> aletter-level and a grapheme-level of processing), indicating that their id<strong>en</strong>tification is mainly and rapidly performedon the basis of whole word orthographic processing. Alternatively, low frequ<strong>en</strong>cy words have a lessstable orthographic repres<strong>en</strong>tation and are thus more affected by the pot<strong>en</strong>tial problems arising in the letter-tophonemetranslation. A similar result is observed wh<strong>en</strong> manipulating the consist<strong>en</strong>cy/regularity of the mappingbetwe<strong>en</strong> sublexical orthography and sublexical phonology (e.g., Cont<strong>en</strong>t, 1991 ; Treiman, Mull<strong>en</strong>nix,Bijeljac-Babic, & Richmond-Welty, 1995 ; Waters & Seid<strong>en</strong>berg, 1985).There may be, however, an alternative interpretation of the pres<strong>en</strong>t phoneme effect. As a matter of fact, bymanipulating the number of phonemes in a word, we also unavoidably changed their syllabic structure : 5-phoneme words had a CCVCC structure, 4-phonemes words had either a CVCC or a CCVC structure, and 3-phoneme words had mostly a CVC structure. Thus, one could argue that processing of 3-phoneme words wasnot inhibited <strong>du</strong>e to competition betwe<strong>en</strong> single- and multi-letter graphemes but rather that the processing ofthe 5-phoneme words was facilitated <strong>du</strong>e to more constraining syllabic structures. Indeed, CCVCC words maybe easier to recognize than CVC words because they are better specified in terms of their phonology and residein a less d<strong>en</strong>se phonological neighborhood. CCVCC words may th<strong>en</strong> activate less competitors in thephonological lexicon compared to CVC words (which are more common structures in English and Fr<strong>en</strong>ch).Therefore, the critical factor in the pres<strong>en</strong>t experim<strong>en</strong>t could be the number of phonological neighbors or, inother terms, the number of graphemes shared by the target word with other lexical <strong>en</strong>tries.Note, however, that there seems to be no indep<strong>en</strong>d<strong>en</strong>t empirical support for the exist<strong>en</strong>ce of aphonological neighborhood effect in visual word recognition. The few studies that have investigated the effectsof phonological neighbors on visual word recognition have reported null effects (Brown & Watson, 1994; Peereman & Cont<strong>en</strong>t, 1997). In contrast, in a study on phonological dyslexia, Derouesne and Beauvois(1979) reported that some of their pati<strong>en</strong>ts exhibited far greater problems wh<strong>en</strong> reading nonwords with multilettergraphemes than wh<strong>en</strong> reading nonwords with single-letter graphemes. In addition, in a manipulationcomparable to ours, Rastle and Coltheart (in press) rec<strong>en</strong>tly reported a similar phoneme effect in nonwordnaming. This effect was pres<strong>en</strong>t in both the human data and the simulations of their <strong>du</strong>al route cascadedmodel. An analysis of the locus of the effect within their simulation model showed that it was <strong>du</strong>e to competitionbetwe<strong>en</strong> multi-letter and single-letter graphemes for priority within the nonlexical route.Thus, our data join those of Rastle and Coltheart and others to suggest that the reading process is influ<strong>en</strong>cedby the fine grained phonographic structure of words. It indicates that word id<strong>en</strong>tification processes ares<strong>en</strong>sitive to the syllabic structure of words and to subsyllabic compon<strong>en</strong>ts such as graphemes. More precisely,the number and the position of graphemes in a word, together with the number of shared graphemes amongdiffer<strong>en</strong>t lexical <strong>en</strong>tries, are factors that seem to critically influ<strong>en</strong>ce the reading process. It thus supports theview according to which « (...) the proper unit of the reading system is neither the single letter nor the wholeword but a higher-order invariant derived from grapheme-phoneme correspond<strong>en</strong>ces » (Gibson, Pick, Osser &Hammond, 1962, p. 570).The idea according to which the reading system develops intermediate processing units <strong>du</strong>ring reading acquisitionhas be<strong>en</strong> discussed at l<strong>en</strong>gth in previous studies. However, there was considerable disagreem<strong>en</strong>t onthe size of these units, that is, if these units should be syllables, morphemes, consonant and vowel clusters,onset and rimes, etc. (for a review of these differ<strong>en</strong>t suggestions, see Rapp, 1992). We will not argue here forthe predominance of a single reading unit. Instead, we favor a « hierarchical » point of view in which differ<strong>en</strong>tsizes of reading units co-exist. These differ<strong>en</strong>t units would emerge <strong>du</strong>ring reading acquisition, with someunits having a primary and more fundam<strong>en</strong>tal role, and other units - g<strong>en</strong>erally of a larger size - being establishedlater (the functional role of these latter units being to increase the automaticity and rapidity of skilledreading).In such a framework, graphemes could be considered as the minimal and primary reading units. Larger andsecondary units may be developed <strong>du</strong>ring the maturation of reading, allowing the reader to detect and recognizewritt<strong>en</strong> words more rapidly. For example, onset and rimes may be possible larger units of the reading system,in English in particular (see Bowey, 1990, 1993 ; Treiman, 1989 ; Treiman & Chafetz, 1987 ; Treiman,Goswami, & Bruck, 1990 ; Treiman et al., 1995 ; Treiman & Zukowski, 1988 ; Wise, Olson, & Treiman,1990). Syllables may also be considered as higher order units, and may ev<strong>en</strong> be more adequate units in Fr<strong>en</strong>ch(see Ferrand, Segui, & Grainger, 1996 ; Ferrand, Segui, & Humphreys, 1996 ; Prinzmetal, Treiman & Rho,1986 ; Rapp, 1992 ; Spoehr & Smith, 1975 ; Taft, 1979). Together, these differ<strong>en</strong>t levels of reading unitswould co-exist in the reading system as stable patterns of letter repres<strong>en</strong>tations. The stability of these patterns

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!