13.07.2015 Views

effet du nombre des graphèmes en Anglais - Aix Marseille Université

effet du nombre des graphèmes en Anglais - Aix Marseille Université

effet du nombre des graphèmes en Anglais - Aix Marseille Université

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

248App<strong>en</strong>dice IAPPENDIXA1. Cleaning Proce<strong>du</strong>re for all 3-,4-, and 5-letter English words tak<strong>en</strong> from theCelex database (Baay<strong>en</strong>, Piep<strong>en</strong>brock & van Rijn, 1993).1) All 3-,4-, and 5-letter English words were extracted from the Celex database2) All words which had a writt<strong>en</strong> (Fwrit) and a spok<strong>en</strong> frequ<strong>en</strong>cy (Fspok) smaller than or equal to 1 per millionwere excluded3) Abbreviations, proper names, etc. were excluded4) Homograph <strong>en</strong>tries were re<strong>du</strong>ced according to the following proce<strong>du</strong>re :-homophonic homographs (e.g., verb/noun : to beat- the beat) were re<strong>du</strong>ced to one single <strong>en</strong>try in theorthographic and phonologic lexicons. Fwrit and Fspok were summed across the multiple homograph<strong>en</strong>tries.- non-homophonic homographs (e.g., lead - lead) were kept as two separate <strong>en</strong>tries in the phonologicallexicon and one <strong>en</strong>try in the orthographic lexicon. Their writt<strong>en</strong> frequ<strong>en</strong>cies (Fwrit) were summed ; theirspok<strong>en</strong> frequ<strong>en</strong>cies (Fspok) were kept separate (not summed).-non-homophonic homograph alternatives with id<strong>en</strong>tical frequ<strong>en</strong>cies but slight variations in their phonologydep<strong>en</strong>ding on regional or contextual constraints were re<strong>du</strong>ced to one standard <strong>en</strong>try in both lexiconsaccording to HARRAP's DICTIONARY. Note : Those multiple <strong>en</strong>tries are alternative pronunciations; they must be separated from real non-homophonic homographs because they have id<strong>en</strong>tical frequ<strong>en</strong>cies.5) For all homophones (e.g., sea - see ; n=444, the spok<strong>en</strong> frequ<strong>en</strong>cies (Fspok) were summed.6) All words with two or more syllables were excluded.7) All words with a grapheme decomposition of 2 were excluded (e.g. mayor, with /m8R/ as phonologicalcode, with "may-or" as graphemic decomposition).This cleaning proce<strong>du</strong>re resulted in an orthographic lexicon consisting of 2494 <strong>en</strong>tries (409 three-letterwords, 1151 four-letter words and 933 five-letter words).A2. Read out proce<strong>du</strong>re used in the simulations.The read out proce<strong>du</strong>re used in these simulation studies follows the <strong>des</strong>ign principles <strong>des</strong>cribed inGrainger and Jacobs (1996). For the Coltheart et al. (1977) and Seid<strong>en</strong>berg et al. (1996) tests, as well as thesecond Stone et al. (in press) test, the critical dep<strong>en</strong>d<strong>en</strong>t variable was correct response time to nonwords. Inthe MROM, a "No" response is g<strong>en</strong>erated wh<strong>en</strong> neither the activation of a lexical unit (µ value) nor theglobal lexical activity (s value) have reached the response criterions (M and ∑, respectively) before thetemporal deadline (T criterion). Thus response time for a "No" response is simply giv<strong>en</strong> by the value of theT criterion. This value is a function of the global lexical activity (s ) g<strong>en</strong>erated by the target stimulus. Ahigh global lexical activity is interpreted as a high probability that the target stimulus is a word. Practically,we assume that <strong>du</strong>ring the early phases of stimulus processing, the computed s value indexes the likelihoodthat the stimulus is a word. A high s value will <strong>en</strong>courage participants to set a longer deadline, that is, ahigher T criterion (Coltheart et al., 1977 ; Jacobs & Grainger, 1992). For the pres<strong>en</strong>t simulations, wemonitored the s value at processing cycle 15 for each stimulus. In the MROM-P simulations, if s (15) >0.33, th<strong>en</strong> T = 27 cycles, else T = 25 cycles. For the MROM simulations, if s (15) > 0.3, th<strong>en</strong> T = 27 cycles,else T = 25 cycles.For the first Stone et al. (1996) test, the critical dep<strong>en</strong>d<strong>en</strong>t variable was correct response time words. Inthe MROM, a "Yes" response is g<strong>en</strong>erated wh<strong>en</strong> either the activation of a lexical unit (µ value) or theglobal lexical activity (s value) have reached the response criterions (M and ∑, respectively) before thetemporal deadline (T criterion). For simplicity, we did not consider in the pres<strong>en</strong>t simulations the possiblerole of global orthographic activity on "Yes" responses. Thus the response time was computed by determiningwh<strong>en</strong> the activation of lexical units reached the ∑ criterion. This criterion was 0.67 for the MROM-Pand 0.47 for the MROM. These values correspond to 90% of the asymptotic activation values for lexicalunits in each model (Jacobs & Grainger, 1992).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!