19.01.2013 Views

S35MZ

S35MZ

S35MZ

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Young babies seem to have different expectations<br />

about words and sounds in some situations,<br />

but when novel sounds were produced<br />

in a joint attention labelling routine, 13-montholds<br />

treated them just as they treated labels.<br />

But the use of symbols others than words in<br />

word learning is not a matter of all or nothing.<br />

Symbolic input during language acquisition can<br />

be advantageous for children who are learning<br />

new labels less tied to contextual familiarity.<br />

McGregor, Rohlfing, Beand and Marschner<br />

(2009) compared the results of using or not using<br />

symbolic input when teaching to a group of<br />

children, aged 1;8-2;0, the spatial term under.<br />

The outcome of this investigation was that children<br />

who received training supplemented with<br />

symbolic gesture demonstrated more robust<br />

long-term gains in word knowledge compared<br />

to children without symbolic support and to<br />

children whose training was supplemented by<br />

symbolic photographs. One possible explanation<br />

proposed by Goldin-Meadow is that gesture<br />

serves to minimize cognitive load during<br />

moments of language processing. One thing<br />

that is clear is that symbolic gestures are ultimately<br />

useful supports for word learning.<br />

Starting from this conclusion, Zammit and<br />

Schafer (2011) put it into practice and observed<br />

how mothers interacted with their infants (0;10)<br />

and how their communicative style affected the<br />

acquisition of target words. Their data suggest<br />

that children of mothers who produce large<br />

numbers of communicative acts learn words<br />

earlier than children of mothers who produced<br />

fewer communicative acts (these conclusions<br />

are valid for the point at which words are understood<br />

but not yet said). So, this paper supports<br />

the previous one (McGregor et al., 2009) proving<br />

that gestures are actually useful in word<br />

learning and an important element of mother’s<br />

communicative behaviour towards their infants.<br />

A very clear conclusion of this section is that<br />

at the earlier stages, children are able to map<br />

words, gestures and non-linguistic sounds, and<br />

that this capacity narrows as the child grows<br />

Word Learning During First Language Acquisition: Linking Sounds to Referents<br />

accustomed to expect words to refer to realities.<br />

However, gestures continue being useful<br />

during the task of words learning, since they<br />

can be used to help children to understand<br />

words which refer to less concrete realities.<br />

Noun vs. Verb learning<br />

There is a long standing assumption, from traditional<br />

part-of-speech analyses, that nouns<br />

are the largest class of words children learn<br />

and, therefore, easier to learn than other classes<br />

of words such as verbs. Bloom, Tinker and<br />

Margulis (1993) studied the actual status of<br />

object words in early vocabularies by observing<br />

children from 9 months to 2 years of age.<br />

The conclusion they reached was that object<br />

names are not privileged for word learning and<br />

that words other than common nouns predominate<br />

in children’s vocabularies (only one third<br />

of the words children learned were names for<br />

things). Furthermore, many children learned relational<br />

words (such as more, up) and socially<br />

mediated expressions (hi, bye, yes), and this<br />

fact proves that a child can override the wholeobject<br />

constraint (Tomasello & Akhtar, 1995). A<br />

possible answer to this fact could be the Principle<br />

of Relevance and other social-interactional<br />

cues (Tomasello & Akhtar, 1995; Grassmann &<br />

Tomasello, 2007).<br />

Golinkoff, Mervis and Hirsh-Pasek (1994)<br />

examined the claim that lexical acquisition proceeds<br />

in the rapid, relatively effortless way that<br />

it does because the child operates with a set<br />

of principles that guide the task of word learning.<br />

According to them, object names make up<br />

the largest proportion of any word type found<br />

in children’s early lexicons and these principles<br />

explain mainly the acquisition of object-words,<br />

not action words.<br />

A more recent study by Brandone, Pence,<br />

Golinkoff and Hirsh-Pasek (2007) explores<br />

how children use two possible solutions to the<br />

verb-mapping problem: attention to perceptually<br />

salient actions and attention to social and<br />

Esdrújula. Revista de filología 135

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!