19.01.2013 Views

S35MZ

S35MZ

S35MZ

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Eva Rodríguez Cárdenas<br />

sounds to a referent. That is, a word is the association<br />

between the sound and the meaning.<br />

Because of that, it is logical that some linguists<br />

have found the phonetic aspect a relevant one<br />

in word learning and have studied it closely.<br />

Some years ago there seemed to be a paradox<br />

in word learning because certain researches<br />

had stated that infants appeared to be abstract<br />

auditory or language processors whereas<br />

toddlers appeared to be non-abstract, item-specific<br />

language users. In order to describe a satisfactory<br />

resolution for these contradictory findings,<br />

Naigles (2002) proposed three resolutions,<br />

being the third the only one relevant for this article.<br />

It gives a simple answer: infancy studies assessed<br />

generalizations only over form, whereas<br />

toddler studies assessed generalizations over<br />

form and meaning. So, the findings of toddlers’<br />

“regression” from being able to abstract patterns<br />

over linguistic stimuli as infants can be attributed<br />

to their problems with meaning, not their<br />

problems with abstract linguistic form. One can<br />

realize from this explanation that infants learn<br />

first the linguistic form and, afterwards, they relate<br />

them to their meaning.<br />

However, there are more questions to be<br />

solved. For example, how do children recognise<br />

single words in fluent speech? For a long<br />

time, it had been assumed that children learnt<br />

words because they heard them in isolation, but<br />

some authors have suggested that children directed<br />

speech (CDS) does not provide enough<br />

isolated words so as to acquire their linguistic<br />

form. In their paper, Brent and Siskind (2001)<br />

revisit the potential role of isolated words in<br />

early word learning. The subjects of the study<br />

were 8 mothers and their children (from 9 to 15<br />

months of age), being each mother-infant pair<br />

recorded while they were alone in their home.<br />

From their study, four main ideas rose. First, isolated<br />

words occur regularly in the experience of<br />

infants. Second, these isolated words comprise<br />

a variety of distinct word types. Third, a significant<br />

proportion of the first words produced by<br />

infants are words typically spoken in isolation<br />

132 Esdrújula. Revista de filología<br />

by their mothers before they start to produce<br />

them. Finally, if a mother speaks a given word in<br />

isolation frequently, it is more probable that her<br />

child will be able to use that word later.<br />

From these results, Brent and Siskind (2001)<br />

concluded that young children typically acquire<br />

a small, initial vocabulary from exposure<br />

to isolated words, but it does not mean that<br />

isolated words are essential, just that it may explain<br />

why children develop language so rapidly.<br />

If this conclusion is true, then infants should<br />

be more efficient in identifying an isolated word<br />

than a word embedded in continuous speech.<br />

That is why Fernald and Hurtado (2006) compared<br />

the responses of 18-month-old infants to<br />

the same words presented in isolation and in<br />

a familiar sentence frame by using online eyetracking<br />

methods, which provide continuous<br />

measures of speed and accuracy that enable<br />

more sensitive assessment of children’s efficiency<br />

in interpreting the same word in different<br />

contexts. The result was that infants were<br />

significantly faster to respond to target words<br />

in continuous speech than to target words in<br />

isolation. This suggests that target word interpretation<br />

was facilitated by the carrier phrase<br />

on sentence-frame trials. Although it may seem<br />

illogical, it is not; one has to take into account<br />

that a word in isolation appears without warning,<br />

whereas familiar frames may enable the<br />

child to anticipate the occurrence of the upcoming<br />

target noun. However, these findings<br />

do not deny completely the conclusions of the<br />

previous paper (Brent & Siskind, 2001), but<br />

points out the importance of the use of short,<br />

simple, repetitive carrier frames leading up to<br />

a familiar noun, especially since this seems to<br />

be a more advantageous habit that the one of<br />

speaking words in isolation.<br />

Words in isolation and frames within the utterances<br />

are not the only cues that children use<br />

when learning new words: an important factor<br />

in language acquisition is infants’ knowledge of<br />

native language sound patterns, as it is shown<br />

in the paper by Estes, Edwards and Saffran

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!