18.02.2013 Views

VOWELS IN STANDARD AUSTRIAN GERMAN - Acoustics ...

VOWELS IN STANDARD AUSTRIAN GERMAN - Acoustics ...

VOWELS IN STANDARD AUSTRIAN GERMAN - Acoustics ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

137<br />

Vowels in Standard Austrian German<br />

The articulators have to fulfill certain requirements to produce a given sequence, and<br />

this process of production is the subject of articulatory phonetics. Yet, Tabain's<br />

conclusion positions the role of acoustic phonetics:<br />

"Although the chapter may initially seem like a "downer" after so many mainstream<br />

articulatory and even theoretical chapters, it is in fact an excellent reminder that the study of<br />

coarticulation is ultimately meaningless unless it can be related to an acoustic output, and<br />

hence to the perception of the speech signal." (Tabain 2001)<br />

The crucial point in the discussion whether acoustic phonetics can contribute something<br />

to the study of coarticulation is, however, the fact that the results of articulatory phone-<br />

tics and acoustic analyses cannot be directly compared. To give a simple example: in a<br />

sequence V1CV2, where C is a bilabial plosive, the tongue is said to be free to move<br />

from V1 to V2, and at the point of release, the tongue is already in the position for V2.<br />

This is impressively shown by the x-ray motion films produced by Wood (1996): in the<br />

sequence /o#pă/ from the Bulgarian utterance "deteto xodi po pătishtata" ("The child<br />

was walking along the path") the uvular tongue body withdrawal of /o/ and the<br />

pharyngeal tongue body approach for /ă/ happen during the occlusion phase of the<br />

bilabial plosive (Wood 1996: 156, Figure 9). Another example is drawn from English,<br />

in which the tongue keeps the position of the vowel [é] and the lips are close for [m],<br />

while the velum is raised for [p] in the utterance "camping" (Kent 1983: 68). From an<br />

articulatory point of view, the mandible and tongue can work independently from one<br />

another to a large extent and two vowels interrupted by a labial consonant rather<br />

perform a diphthongal lingual movement. For this reason, some researchers restrict their<br />

analyses on vowel to vowel coarticulation on sequences in which the consonant(s) are<br />

bilabial (e.g. Manuel 1990, Magen 1997, Cho 2004) 97 .<br />

"The first and second consonants (C1, C2) were always /b/, whose articulation is known to<br />

interfere minimally with the vocalic lingual articulation." (Cho 2004: 146)<br />

However, diphthongal tongue movement during a bilabial closure is not undisputed. So-<br />

called troughs (deactivation of the tongue musculature during the bilabial closure) in<br />

97 Acoustic analyses have been performed by Manuel (1990) and Magen (1997). The study<br />

performed by Cho (2004) is an articulatory one.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!