29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 3044–3050 Friday Evening<br />

pus, MATTHEW G. RHODES, Washington University, & HARPREET<br />

BAHIA, Arizona State University, West Campus—<strong>The</strong> present study<br />

investigated the impact of memory monitoring on false recognition in<br />

the DRM paradigm (Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995).<br />

Participants were given either a recall test or an opposition task immediately<br />

after each list presentation. In the opposition task, participants<br />

were instructed to write down words that were related to the presented<br />

word list but that had not themselves been previously<br />

presented. <strong>The</strong> opposition task was employed to encourage memory<br />

monitoring. Participants were later given a yes/no recognition test in<br />

order to assess memory for the presented lists. Results indicated that<br />

the opposition task was effective at reducing false memories, but only<br />

for those critical lures that were identified as nonpresented items during<br />

encoding.<br />

(3044)<br />

Does Distinctive Visual Information Enhance Monitoring of False<br />

Memories? JASON ARNDT, Middlebury College, ASHLEY OS-<br />

BORNE, Duke University, & MEGHAN STONE, JOHN CARNEY, &<br />

KAREN LEE, Middlebury College—Two experiments evaluated the<br />

role of encoding-based and retrieval-based factors in the production<br />

of false recognition in the DRM paradigm. <strong>The</strong> first experiment varied<br />

the association of unusual-looking fonts with DRM themes (correlated<br />

or not correlated) and the duration of time allotted for subjects<br />

to utilize retrieval monitoring. <strong>The</strong> second experiment manipulated<br />

whether the font in which an unstudied lure item was presented at test<br />

was used to present strong or weak associates of the lure, as measured<br />

by backward associative strength. <strong>The</strong> combined results of these experiments<br />

suggest that subjects utilize visual format as a retrieval cue<br />

in an effort to distinguish between studied and unstudied items but<br />

that this monitoring strategy can be harmful when lure activation during<br />

encoding leads to the development of associations between fonts<br />

used to present study items and lure item representations.<br />

(3045)<br />

False Recall in the DRM Paradigm for Word and/or Pseudohomophone<br />

Lists. MICHAEL J. CORTESE & ILIJA VELJKOVIC, College<br />

of Charleston, & MAYA M. KHANNA, University of Michigan—<strong>The</strong><br />

role of plausibility in false recall in the DRM paradigm was examined<br />

using free recall. Presentation lists consisted of words, pseudohomophones<br />

(e.g., dreem), or both words and pseudohomophones. When<br />

participants were instructed to write down items as they appeared during<br />

study (Experiment 1), the proportion of false recall was higher for<br />

both word and mixed lists than for pseudohomophone lists. However,<br />

in verbal recall (Experiment 2), the three list conditions produced similar<br />

levels of false recall. We posit that Experiment 1 yielded less false<br />

recall for pseudohomophone lists because items activated in memory<br />

were correctly spelled and, thus, represented implausible events in a<br />

pseudohomophone context. Interestingly, in the pseudohomophone<br />

condition, when participants did report critical items, they usually<br />

spelled them as pseudohomophones. In Experiment 2, critical items<br />

activated in memory were indistinguishable from list words in the<br />

pseudohomophone condition, because the task probed verbal, rather<br />

than orthographic, information.<br />

(3046)<br />

Differences in False Recognition From DRM and Categorized Lists:<br />

Associative Activation or Gist? BENTON H. PIERCE, Texas A&M<br />

University, Commerce, DAVID A. GALLO, University of Chicago,&<br />

DANIEL L. SCHACTER, Harvard University—False recognition<br />

from DRM lists (e.g., core, orchard, pie, . . .) is consistently found to<br />

be greater than that from categorized lists (e.g., pear, peach, orange,<br />

. . .). To test whether this difference (DRM > categorized) is attributable<br />

to differences in associative strength or to differences in semantic<br />

gist, we created DRM lists and categorized lists that were related<br />

to the same critical lure (e.g., apple) and were equated on backward<br />

associative strength (via association norms). Using a standard old/new<br />

recognition test, we found equivalent levels of false recognition for the<br />

94<br />

two list types. A meaning recognition test, however, revealed that critical<br />

lures from categorized lists cued the gist of the list more frequently<br />

than did DRM critical lures. This interaction supports the idea<br />

that stronger associative connections, as opposed to greater semantic<br />

gist, are responsible for the greater false recognition levels commonly<br />

found for DRM-type lists than for categorized lists.<br />

(3047)<br />

Impact of Exposure to Evidence on False Confessions to Actions.<br />

MICHELLE CARBUTO & LINDA A. HENKEL, Fairfield University—<br />

Certain interrogation tactics (e.g., showing suspects pictures from a<br />

crime scene) can contribute to the incidence of false confessions.<br />

Using an experimental paradigm modeled after “imagination inflation”<br />

studies, this experiment investigated the impact of the presentation<br />

of false evidence on people’s memory for which actions were actually<br />

performed. Subjects performed and imagined performing<br />

various actions that would leave behind evidence that the action had<br />

been completed (e.g., tear the card in half, crumple the paper). Later,<br />

they were exposed to photographs (zero, one, or three times) showing<br />

some of the imagined actions in a state of completion. One week later,<br />

they had to indicate which actions had been actually performed. Results<br />

indicated that the presentation of evidence increased source errors<br />

for imagined events: Subjects were more likely to falsely claim<br />

to have performed actions that were in fact imagined when photographic<br />

evidence of the completed actions had been shown.<br />

(3048)<br />

Emotion Above Semantic Relatedness: Explaining Memory Illusion.<br />

LILIAN M. STEIN, GUSTAVO ROHENKOHL, & RENATO F. SAN-<br />

TOS, Pontifical Catholic University, Rio Grande do Sul—Semantic<br />

relatedness has been used to explain false memory phenomena in the<br />

Deese/Roediger–McDermott paradigm. <strong>The</strong> present experiment<br />

tested the effect of emotional valence on memory illusion independently<br />

of semantic relatedness. Two hundred eighty-five Brazilian college<br />

students were presented with 12 lists of semantic associates (3<br />

positive, 6 neutral, 3 negative). Semantic relatedness (backward and<br />

forward association strength) was equated across the three emotional<br />

valence groups of lists. Results indicated that emotional valence<br />

seemed to produce a differential effect on true and false recognition.<br />

An immediate memory test yielded more false recognition responses<br />

to critical nonpresented associate words in negative lists than to those<br />

in both neutral and positive lists. Turning to veridical memory, participants<br />

produced higher hit rates for presented words from both positive<br />

and negative lists, as compared with neutral lists. <strong>The</strong> differential<br />

effect of emotional valence on true and false recognition is<br />

explained in light of current theories of memory.<br />

(3049)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of Gesture on Memory and the Creation of False Memories.<br />

JENNIFER L. TOMES & KAREN G. NICHOLSON, Mount<br />

Allison University—In the Deese/Roediger–McDermott paradigm, subsequent<br />

to learning semantically related words (bed, dream, etc.), participants<br />

falsely remember other semantically related words not on the<br />

original lists (sleep; i.e., “critical lures”). We examined whether producing<br />

gestures that relate to each word during learning would reduce<br />

false memory for critical lures. Participants (n = 150) were presented<br />

with 10 lists of 10 semantically related words. Half the participants were<br />

asked to generate a related gesture for each word. Subsequently, participants<br />

were asked to make old/new discriminations for items on a test<br />

list. Results showed that the gesture group correctly recognized significantly<br />

more words that were actually on the lists than did the no-gesture<br />

group. In contrast, the no-gesture group falsely recognized significantly<br />

more critical lures than did the gesture group. <strong>The</strong> results are discussed<br />

in terms of how gesture production might influence the activation of<br />

semantic networks thought to mediate the false memory effects.<br />

(3050)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effects of Retrieval Practice on False Recall. KIMBERLY K.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!