Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Posters 3030–3035 Friday Evening University of Tamagawa, & NOBUO OHTA, Tokyo University of Social Welfare—Raw scores on intelligence tests have been rising at a fast rate, especially for older adults. One explanation is that increases in intelligence parallel increases in educational attainment. In Study 1, we searched for studies reporting WAIS–R Vocabulary raw scores from January 1986 to December 2003 and plotted raw scores by publication year. From the 1960s, verbal intelligence scores for older adults have risen sharply, until reaching test ceilings in the 1990s. In contrast, scores for younger adults have risen only minimally and have begun to decline. In Study 2, we confirmed substantial verbal intelligence declines in young adults over the last decade. In combination, VIQ score trajectories mimic the trajectories of educational attainment by the two age groups, combined with anecdotal evidence of decline in education quality. These results suggest that the days of the Flynn effect, at least for verbal intelligence, may be over: it’s all downhill from here. (3030) Adult Age Differences in Episodic and Semantic Retrieval: A Diffusion Model Analysis. JULIA SPANIOL & DAVID J. MADDEN, Duke University Medical Center, ANDREAS VOSS, Albert-Ludwigs- Universität Freiburg, & SARA E. MOORE, Duke University Medical Center—This study investigated adult age differences in episodic and semantic long-term memory retrieval, as a test of the hypothesis of specific age-related decline in context memory. In Experiment 1, old–new recognition served as an episodic retrieval task, and living/ nonliving decisions served as a semantic retrieval task. Older adults were slower and had lower episodic accuracy than did younger adults. Fits of the diffusion model (Ratcliff, 1978) revealed age-related increases in the length of nondecisional response time components for both episodic and semantic retrieval. There was also an age-related decrease in the rate of accumulation of decision-driving information (drift rate) for episodic retrieval, but not for semantic retrieval. A substantial portion of the age-related variance in episodic drift rate was shared with perceptual–motor speed. A second experiment contrasted episodic and semantic retrieval mechanisms, using a more contextdependent episodic task (source memory judgments). (3031) The Influence of Age and Sport Expertise on Memory for Visual, Verbal, and Enacted Information. CLARE MACMAHON, KATINKA DIJKSTRA, & MINE MISIRLISOY, Florida State University—Subjectperformed tasks (SPTs), such as the action of waving, are recalled better than verbal instructions (“wave”) and show smaller age-related differences (Bäckman, 1985). This study assessed whether previous experience with certain motor actions further facilitate or attenuate the SPT effect. Forty-eight older and younger experienced golfers and 48 older and younger control participants were compared in their memory for golf-related (e.g., “putt to the hole”) and non–golf-related (e.g., “turn on the lamp”) items. Items were presented verbally (read aloud) or visually (video clip) or were subject performed. Preliminary findings show an interaction between information type and expertise in retention. Specifically, experienced golfers remember more golfrelated information than do controls. As well, older adults demonstrate smaller declines in delayed retention for golf-related information than do younger adults. These findings support the idea that experienced golfers develop elaborate perceptual–motor knowledge specific to their area of expertise (Starkes, Cullen, & MacMahon, 2004). • AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY • (3032) Verifiable Autobiographical Memory: Recall of Lies. STEPHANIE A. BERGER, College of Mount St. Vincent—This diary study of autobiographical memory examined the accuracy with which students recalled their own lies. Students submitted each lie they had told every day for 7 days to a confidential online diary. Along with each lie, they 92 also recorded the reason for the lie and completed subjective rating scales, including the seriousness of the lie, the amount of advanced planning before telling the lie, and their feelings about the lie. Participants recalled .81 of all lies on an unexpected cued-recall test that was completed in the lab 2 weeks after the last lie had been submitted. Significant differences between the characteristics of the lies that were recalled and of those that were not provide evidence of the functional nature of autobiographical memory. (3033) Remembrance of French Things Past. LILLIAN H. PARK & JOHN F. KIHLSTROM, University of California, Berkeley—Two experiments with French–English bilinguals investigated the relationship between language and memory. When subjects were asked to write in French, they retrieved more memories from the period in their lives when they spoke French; when they wrote in English, they retrieved more memories from the period in which English dominated their speech. Autobiographical memories narrated in a different language from that in which the events had been experienced did not have fewer details than did those told in the same language. Autobiographical memories that were narrated in the same language in which the event had been experienced were more expressive and emotional than those narrated in the other language. Bilingual speakers retrieved and preferred to remember autobiographical memories in the same language in which the event had been experienced. Language acquisition history and habitual use of the two languages affected the strength of these language effects. (3034) The Reminiscence Bump in Autobiographical Memory: Effects of Age and Culture. STEVE M. J. JANSSEN & JEROEN G. W. RAAIJ- MAKERS, University of Amsterdam—We investigated the age distribution of autobiographical memories with the Galton–Crovitz cuing method through the Internet. Participants from different countries, such as Australia, Belgium, Canada, the Netherlands, the U.K., and the U.S., were presented with 10 cue words. They were asked to recall personal memories that were associated to the cue words and to date these personal events. We were able to remove the recency effect from the empirical age distributions with a method that allows separate estimation of memory encoding and forgetting. We found strong evidence for a “reminiscence bump” in all subpopulations at all ages. People stored more events (or they store events better) between the ages of 10 and 20 years. However, we also found that the reminiscence bump becomes more pronounced as participants become older. Events that occurred between the ages of 10 and 20 years are recalled more often, making them even more persistent. (3035) The Specificity and Organization of Autobiographical Memory. MATTHEW D. SCHULKIND, Amherst College, TAMARA A. RAH- HAL, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, & MEGAN R. KLEIN & SAMANTHA R. LACHER, Amherst College—Younger adult subjects were asked to describe autobiographical events in response to cue narratives that varied in terms of general life period, emotional valence, and theme. Sixty percent of the subjects’ narratives described specific, on-one-day events, and nearly 40% described specific episodes within such events. As well, the subjects’ narratives matched the cue narratives in terms of theme and valence more often than they matched them in terms of general life period. These data contradict previous research showing that people tend to report general events spanning several days, rather than specific events that occurred on a single day. The data also contradict theories arguing that autobiographical knowledge is organized hierarchically, with general life periods anchoring the top of the hierarchy. Therefore, current theories must be altered to account for the fact that retrieval cues/conditions influence both the way autobiographical knowledge is accessed and the specificity of the events retrieved from memory.

Friday Evening Posters 3036–3043 (3036) Retellings of Autobiographical Memories. MICHAEL B. HUTSON & ELIZABETH J. MARSH, Duke University (sponsored by Ian Dobbins)— People tell their autobiographical memories to different people and for different reasons. In lab studies, retelling a story to a different audience or for a different purpose changes what (and how much) information is included (Hyman, 1994; Marsh, Tversky, & Hutson, in press). The present research examines whether retellings of personal memories also show evidence of shifting for different goals. Study 1 involved collection of scripts for typical events and application of the scripts to retellings told to entertain. Stories contained fewer actions, but what actions were mentioned conformed to script predictions. Study 2 involved collection of typical instances of events (to provide converging evidence for the scripts collected in Study 1) and retellings of personal memories to inform versus to entertain. Informative and entertaining retellings differed in proportion of script events included, as well as the language used. • FALSE MEMORY • (3037) Effects of Bilingual Processing on Veridical and False Memory. YVONNE WAKEFORD, Tufts University, MICHAEL T. CARLIN, University of Massachusetts, Worcester, & MICHAEL P. TOGLIA, SUNY, Cortland—The Deese/Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigm was utilized to assess the effect of bilingual processing on veridical and false memory. Thirty-two English–Spanish bilingual participants were tested in two within-language conditions, with acquisition and test in the same language (English–English and Spanish–Spanish), and two crosslanguage conditions, with acquisition and test in a different language (English–Spanish and Spanish–English). In each condition, participants were presented with 48 words (four lists of 12 associated words). A free recall test was followed by a recognition test. Results indicated a significant interaction; veridical recall and recognition rates were lower in the cross-language conditions, and false recall and recognition rates were higher in the cross-language conditions. This pattern of results indicates that bilingual processing has an adverse effect on memory accuracy and is consistent with spreading-activation theory and gist processing. (3038) The (Un)Reliability of False Memories. MANILA VANNUCCI, University of Florence, & GIULIANA MAZZONI, University of Plymouth (sponsored by Giuliana Mazzoni)—False memories are usually displayed only in a subset of participants. This has led to a search for the correlates of false memory development. Underlying this search is the tacit assumption that individual differences in false memories are reliable. Blair et al. (2002) reported adequate test–retest reliability of false memories for semantically related words (DRM). In a follow-up study, we found significant test–retest reliability only when the same set of DRM lists was presented in the same order. These data suggest that the apparent reliability of DRM false memories may be a contextually mediated artifact. (3039) The Role of Sublexical Activation in Phonological False Memories for Nonwords. MCKENZIE R. BALLOU & MITCHELL S. SOMMERS, Washington University (sponsored by David A. Balota)—Many accounts of false memories in the Deese/Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigm include the assumption that activation accrues on lexical representations of the critical item (CI). However, recent findings that false memories can be produced using lists of nonwords phonologically related to a nonword CI suggest that activation on sublexical, rather than lexical, representations may also contribute to false remembering in the DRM paradigm. To test this hypothesis, two experiments were conducted in which participants’ memory for lists of aurally presented nonwords differing in phonotactic probability (PP) was tested. The facilitative effects of PP (faster and more accurate responding for high than for low PP) for nonword processing are thought to reflect sub- 93 lexical activation, because the unit of analysis in computing PP is either positional or biphone frequency. Results suggest that false memories for nonwords vary as a function of PP, with higher PP producing increased rates of false memory. (3040) Effects of Associative Processes on False Memory: Evidence From DRM and Category Lists. MINE MISIRLISOY, Florida State University, & HASAN G. TEKMAN, Middle East Technical University—The study investigated the effects of test-induced priming on false memories evoked by converging associates (DRM lists) and category associates (category lists) procedures. Participants (mean age = 19.65) were given 18 lists of 12 items to study. This was followed by a recognition test of 297 items, composed of 216 list items, 36 critical items, and 45 filler items. The experiment involved the manipulation of the test order of the critical items in relation to the list items from their corresponding lists. The subjective experience accompanying the memory was also measured by remember/know judgments. The results demonstrated a significant effect of list type (category vs. DRM) in false memories. Critical lures from DRM lists had higher proportions of false memory than did those from category lists, and they received more remember judgments. Results are discussed with respect to the differential effects of encoding and retrieval processes on DRM and category lists. (3041) Heuristic Influences on the False Recollection of Imagined Events. HEATHER M. KLEIDER, Georgia State University, & STEPHEN D. GOLDINGER, Arizona State University—When recollection is difficult, people rely on heuristic processing, rather than on effortful processing. Heuristics increases reliance on perceptual cues when the source of retrieved information is determined. As such, perceptual elaboration of suggested event details is likely to increase false memories (Drivdahl & Zaragoza, 2001). Extending these findings to person memory, this study investigated whether heuristic processing increases false alarms to imagined events by manipulating the delay between witnessing and recalling an event and the stereotype-consistency of a person’s role in the event. In two experiments, participants watched slide shows and imagined people performing stereotype-consistent and -inconsistent actions, followed by immediate or delayed memory tests. Results showed that after a delay, people more often remembered seeing imagined actions and misremembered the stereotypeconsistent person as the actor. In addition, falsely remembering the stereotype-consistent actor was more often attributed to actual memory in a remember/know test. (3042) Lexical Association and False Memory in Two Cultures. YUH- SHIOW LEE & HSU-CHING HUNG, National Chung Cheng University—This study examined the relationship between language experience and false memory produced by the DRM paradigm. The word lists used in Stadler et al. (1999) were first translated into Chinese. False recall and false recognition for critical nonpresented targets were tested on a group of Chinese users. The average co-occurrence rate of the list word and the critical word was calculated on the basis of a large Chinese corpus. List-level analyses revealed that the correlation between the American and the Taiwanese participants was significant only in false recognition. The co-occurrence rate was significantly correlated with false recall and recognition of Taiwanese participants, and not of American participants. On the other hand, the BAS based on Nelson et al. (1999) was significantly correlated with false recall of American participants, and not of Taiwanese participants. Results are discussed in terms of the relationship between language experiences and lexical association in creating false memory. (3043) Memory Monitoring Reduces, but Does Not Eliminate, False Memories. JEFFREY S. ANASTASI, Arizona State University, West Cam-

Posters 3030–3035 Friday Evening<br />

University of Tamagawa, & NOBUO OHTA, Tokyo University of Social<br />

Welfare—Raw scores on intelligence tests have been rising at a<br />

fast rate, especially for older adults. One explanation is that increases<br />

in intelligence parallel increases in educational attainment. In Study 1,<br />

we searched for studies reporting WAIS–R Vocabulary raw scores<br />

from January 1986 to December 2003 and plotted raw scores by publication<br />

year. From the 1960s, verbal intelligence scores for older<br />

adults have risen sharply, until reaching test ceilings in the 1990s. In<br />

contrast, scores for younger adults have risen only minimally and have<br />

begun to decline. In Study 2, we confirmed substantial verbal intelligence<br />

declines in young adults over the last decade. In combination,<br />

VIQ score trajectories mimic the trajectories of educational attainment<br />

by the two age groups, combined with anecdotal evidence of decline<br />

in education quality. <strong>The</strong>se results suggest that the days of the<br />

Flynn effect, at least for verbal intelligence, may be over: it’s all downhill<br />

from here.<br />

(3030)<br />

Adult Age Differences in Episodic and Semantic Retrieval: A Diffusion<br />

Model Analysis. JULIA SPANIOL & DAVID J. MADDEN,<br />

Duke University Medical Center, ANDREAS VOSS, Albert-Ludwigs-<br />

Universität Freiburg, & SARA E. MOORE, Duke University Medical<br />

Center—This study investigated adult age differences in episodic and<br />

semantic long-term memory retrieval, as a test of the hypothesis of<br />

specific age-related decline in context memory. In Experiment 1,<br />

old–new recognition served as an episodic retrieval task, and living/<br />

nonliving decisions served as a semantic retrieval task. Older adults<br />

were slower and had lower episodic accuracy than did younger adults.<br />

Fits of the diffusion model (Ratcliff, 1978) revealed age-related increases<br />

in the length of nondecisional response time components for<br />

both episodic and semantic retrieval. <strong>The</strong>re was also an age-related<br />

decrease in the rate of accumulation of decision-driving information<br />

(drift rate) for episodic retrieval, but not for semantic retrieval. A substantial<br />

portion of the age-related variance in episodic drift rate was<br />

shared with perceptual–motor speed. A second experiment contrasted<br />

episodic and semantic retrieval mechanisms, using a more contextdependent<br />

episodic task (source memory judgments).<br />

(3031)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Influence of Age and Sport Expertise on Memory for Visual,<br />

Verbal, and Enacted Information. CLARE MACMAHON, KATINKA<br />

DIJKSTRA, & MINE MISIRLISOY, Florida State University—Subjectperformed<br />

tasks (SPTs), such as the action of waving, are recalled better<br />

than verbal instructions (“wave”) and show smaller age-related differences<br />

(Bäckman, 1985). This study assessed whether previous experience<br />

with certain motor actions further facilitate or attenuate the<br />

SPT effect. Forty-eight older and younger experienced golfers and 48<br />

older and younger control participants were compared in their memory<br />

for golf-related (e.g., “putt to the hole”) and non–golf-related<br />

(e.g., “turn on the lamp”) items. Items were presented verbally (read<br />

aloud) or visually (video clip) or were subject performed. Preliminary<br />

findings show an interaction between information type and expertise<br />

in retention. Specifically, experienced golfers remember more golfrelated<br />

information than do controls. As well, older adults demonstrate<br />

smaller declines in delayed retention for golf-related information than<br />

do younger adults. <strong>The</strong>se findings support the idea that experienced<br />

golfers develop elaborate perceptual–motor knowledge specific to<br />

their area of expertise (Starkes, Cullen, & MacMahon, 2004).<br />

• AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY •<br />

(3032)<br />

Verifiable Autobiographical Memory: Recall of Lies. STEPHANIE A.<br />

BERGER, College of Mount St. Vincent—This diary study of autobiographical<br />

memory examined the accuracy with which students recalled<br />

their own lies. Students submitted each lie they had told every<br />

day for 7 days to a confidential online diary. Along with each lie, they<br />

92<br />

also recorded the reason for the lie and completed subjective rating<br />

scales, including the seriousness of the lie, the amount of advanced<br />

planning before telling the lie, and their feelings about the lie. Participants<br />

recalled .81 of all lies on an unexpected cued-recall test that<br />

was completed in the lab 2 weeks after the last lie had been submitted.<br />

Significant differences between the characteristics of the lies that<br />

were recalled and of those that were not provide evidence of the functional<br />

nature of autobiographical memory.<br />

(3033)<br />

Remembrance of French Things Past. LILLIAN H. PARK & JOHN F.<br />

KIHLSTROM, University of California, Berkeley—Two experiments<br />

with French–English bilinguals investigated the relationship between<br />

language and memory. When subjects were asked to write in French,<br />

they retrieved more memories from the period in their lives when they<br />

spoke French; when they wrote in English, they retrieved more memories<br />

from the period in which English dominated their speech. Autobiographical<br />

memories narrated in a different language from that in<br />

which the events had been experienced did not have fewer details than<br />

did those told in the same language. Autobiographical memories that<br />

were narrated in the same language in which the event had been experienced<br />

were more expressive and emotional than those narrated in<br />

the other language. Bilingual speakers retrieved and preferred to remember<br />

autobiographical memories in the same language in which<br />

the event had been experienced. Language acquisition history and habitual<br />

use of the two languages affected the strength of these language<br />

effects.<br />

(3034)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Reminiscence Bump in Autobiographical Memory: Effects of<br />

Age and Culture. STEVE M. J. JANSSEN & JEROEN G. W. RAAIJ-<br />

MAKERS, University of Amsterdam—We investigated the age distribution<br />

of autobiographical memories with the Galton–Crovitz cuing<br />

method through the Internet. Participants from different countries,<br />

such as Australia, Belgium, Canada, the Netherlands, the U.K., and<br />

the U.S., were presented with 10 cue words. <strong>The</strong>y were asked to recall<br />

personal memories that were associated to the cue words and to<br />

date these personal events. We were able to remove the recency effect<br />

from the empirical age distributions with a method that allows separate<br />

estimation of memory encoding and forgetting. We found strong<br />

evidence for a “reminiscence bump” in all subpopulations at all ages.<br />

People stored more events (or they store events better) between the<br />

ages of 10 and 20 years. However, we also found that the reminiscence<br />

bump becomes more pronounced as participants become older. Events<br />

that occurred between the ages of 10 and 20 years are recalled more<br />

often, making them even more persistent.<br />

(3035)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Specificity and Organization of Autobiographical Memory.<br />

MATTHEW D. SCHULKIND, Amherst College, TAMARA A. RAH-<br />

HAL, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, & MEGAN R. KLEIN &<br />

SAMANTHA R. LACHER, Amherst College—Younger adult subjects<br />

were asked to describe autobiographical events in response to cue narratives<br />

that varied in terms of general life period, emotional valence,<br />

and theme. Sixty percent of the subjects’ narratives described specific,<br />

on-one-day events, and nearly 40% described specific episodes<br />

within such events. As well, the subjects’ narratives matched the cue<br />

narratives in terms of theme and valence more often than they<br />

matched them in terms of general life period. <strong>The</strong>se data contradict<br />

previous research showing that people tend to report general events<br />

spanning several days, rather than specific events that occurred on a<br />

single day. <strong>The</strong> data also contradict theories arguing that autobiographical<br />

knowledge is organized hierarchically, with general life periods<br />

anchoring the top of the hierarchy. <strong>The</strong>refore, current theories<br />

must be altered to account for the fact that retrieval cues/conditions<br />

influence both the way autobiographical knowledge is accessed and<br />

the specificity of the events retrieved from memory.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!