29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Friday Evening Posters 3023–3029<br />

tion. One assumption about the internal clock is that it can be started<br />

and stopped arbitrarily, much like a stopwatch. <strong>The</strong> present study<br />

tested this assumption in two experiments. Participants first learned<br />

to reproduce 1-sec, 2-sec, or 4-sec target durations, using a fixedinterval<br />

training procedure, and then were tested on their ability to reproduce<br />

the target duration in the presence of a perturbation tone.<br />

Under run instructions, participants ignored the perturbation tone and<br />

responded as during training. Under reset instructions, participants<br />

restarted timing following the perturbation tone. Inconsistent with the<br />

arbitrary start/stop assumption, temporal productions under run and<br />

reset instructions were strongly affected by the phase of the perturbation<br />

and were dependent on the target duration.<br />

(3023)<br />

Timing of Cross-Modal Intervals in the Ranges of Milliseconds and<br />

Seconds: Evidence for a Single Mechanism of Temporal Information<br />

Processing. THOMAS RAMMSAYER, University of Göttingen, &<br />

JUDITH NITSCHKE & ROLF ULRICH, University of Tübingen—<br />

Several lines of research suggest two distinct timing mechanisms underlying<br />

temporal processing (distinct timing hypothesis): a sensory<br />

mechanism for processing of durations in the subsecond range and a<br />

cognitive one for processing of longer durations. In three experiments,<br />

subjects discriminated between the duration of a standard stimulus<br />

and the duration of a variable comparison stimulus. On congruent trials,<br />

both stimuli were presented to the same sensory modality (i.e.,<br />

both visual or both auditory), whereas on incongruent trials, each<br />

stimulus was presented to a different modality. According to the distinct<br />

timing hypothesis, sensory incongruency should lower temporal<br />

discrimination performance for brief, but not for long, standards. Contrary<br />

to this prediction, sensory compatibility affected performance<br />

more for long than for short durations. It is concluded that a singleclock<br />

mechanism based on neural counting provides a better account<br />

for these results than does the distinct timing hypothesis.<br />

(3024)<br />

Interruption in Time Reproduction: An Analysis of Interference Between<br />

Specific Processes. LOBNA CHÉRIF & CLAUDETTE FORTIN,<br />

Université Laval (sponsored by Claudette Fortin)—<strong>The</strong> impact of an<br />

interruption on a time reproduction task involving the encoding and<br />

reproduction of time intervals was investigated in this experiment.<br />

Participants were trained to reproduce time intervals during which a<br />

tone was presented. <strong>The</strong> tone was interrupted either during the encoding<br />

or during the reproduction phase of the task. Participants were<br />

instructed to reproduce the tone duration while ignoring the time for<br />

which it was interrupted. Results show that expecting the interruption<br />

causes an underestimation of the tone duration but that the effect was<br />

stronger when the interruption occurred in the reproduction phase of<br />

the task than when it occurred in the encoding phase. <strong>The</strong>se findings<br />

suggest that the effect of expecting an interruption in timing varies,<br />

depending on the specific operations required in the timing task, and<br />

they are discussed within a framework that emphasizes the processes involved<br />

in the encoding and reproduction phases of time reproduction.<br />

• AGING AND MEMORY •<br />

(3025)<br />

Aging and Qualitative Characteristics of Memories. SUZANNE M.<br />

BLOISE, MARCIA K. JOHNSON, CAROL L. RAYE, & KAREN J.<br />

MITCHELL, Yale University, & LINDA M. BARTOSHUK & DEREK J.<br />

SNYDER, Yale University School of Medicine—We investigated age<br />

differences in memory for complex pictures, using objective and subjective<br />

memory measures. In preliminary analyses, given labels of<br />

pictures, older adults showed poorer old/new recognition than did<br />

younger adults, but gave higher ratings on some subjective memory<br />

measures. We compare memory characteristic questionnaires (McQ),<br />

using a typical McQ rating scale (Johnson et al., 1988) or using a variant<br />

of the general linear magnitude scale (Bartoshuk et al., 2002), and<br />

91<br />

discuss their relative advantages and disadvantages for comparing<br />

across age groups and types of features of memory.<br />

(3026)<br />

Age, Implicit Motives, and Memory for Positive and Negative Social<br />

Information. ANGELA H. GUTCHESS, Harvard University, & CARO-<br />

LYN YOON & OLIVER C. SCHULTHEISS, University of Michigan—<br />

Elderly adults exhibit poor memory for negative emotional information,<br />

but less impaired memory for positive emotional information<br />

(Charles, Mather, & Carstensen, 2003). We tested whether this finding<br />

extends to social information. Sixty young and 60 elderly adults<br />

incidentally encoded 60 words and 90 pictures that were neutral, positive<br />

social (e.g., “together”; depictions of group interactions), or negative<br />

social (e.g., “apart”; depictions of isolated individuals). Positive<br />

and negative social pictures, but not words, were better recognized,<br />

relative to control pictures, for young and elderly adults, but elderly<br />

adults were not disproportionately impaired on negative social stimuli.<br />

However, elderly adults more motivated by affiliation and intimacy<br />

(as assessed by the Picture Story Exercise; Schultheiss & Brunstein,<br />

2001) showed a greater relative memory advantage for social<br />

stimuli, relative to control stimuli. <strong>The</strong> relationship was not present<br />

for young adults, suggesting that individual differences in motivation<br />

have a greater contribution to memory with age.<br />

(3027)<br />

Collaborative Facilitation in High-Functioning Older Adults.<br />

MICHELLE L. MEADE & DENISE C. PARK, University of Illinois,<br />

Urbana-Champaign—We tested the hypothesis that retrieval strategy<br />

and repeated trials would reduce collaborative inhibition in young and<br />

older adults. Subjects recalled unrelated word lists on their own or in<br />

collaboration with another subject and developed strategies (individually<br />

or collaboratively) to improve memory across repeated study–test<br />

trials. Collaborative inhibition obtained on the first recall trial for both<br />

young and older adults but persisted across trials only for young adults<br />

(whose individual memory did not decline). For older adults, individual<br />

recall performance declined across trials, but collaborative recall<br />

performance did not decline. This pattern of collaborative facilitation<br />

was obtained only for high-functioning older adults; the mechanism<br />

for collaborative facilitation was the suggestion of more effective<br />

strategies (not necessarily collaborative strategies) that require a<br />

baseline level of cognitive capacity to implement. In cases where individual<br />

memory declines, it may be advantageous to look to a collaborator<br />

for more effective retrieval strategies.<br />

(3028)<br />

Retrieving the Names of Cartoon Characters: Effects of Name Descriptiveness<br />

and Aging. LORI E. JAMES & KETHERA A. FOGLER,<br />

University of Colorado, Colorado Springs—Few studies have tested<br />

why a person’s name is more difficult to retrieve than other information<br />

about that person. Names usually lack meaningful reference to individuals,<br />

and this has been suggested to contribute to the difficulty<br />

of names. Large age differences have been identified in memory for<br />

names, and this experiment tested whether the arbitrary assignment of<br />

names to individuals contributes to the age decrement in name memory.<br />

Young and older adults named pictures of cartoon characters with<br />

descriptive (depicting personality or appearance; e.g., Pink Panther)<br />

and nondescriptive (e.g., Garfield) names. For percent correct, older<br />

adults demonstrated pronounced deficits in recalling nondescriptive<br />

names, as compared with descriptive ones. This interaction suggests<br />

that part of the often-obtained age deficit in name memory occurs because<br />

names generally lack meaningful reference to individuals.<br />

Findings are interpreted within current theories of cognitive aging,<br />

and practical applications of the results are suggested.<br />

(3029)<br />

Verbal Intelligence: Over the Hill and Picking Up Speed. AMY L.<br />

SIEGENTHALER, Tokyo University of Social Welfare, BOB UTTL,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!