29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 3015–3022 Friday Evening<br />

familiar toys and transparent boxes, either empty or containing familiar<br />

objects, and (2) that included separate balls, pairs of balls with<br />

rigid or flexible connectors (dumbbells), and balls in bags. Children<br />

never counted filled containers, neither transparent boxes nor bags.<br />

Adults consistently counted both filled and empty transparent boxes;<br />

some also counted filled bags. Children did count empty containers<br />

if they were the only objects presented on a trial and on trials following<br />

all empty-container trials. Children never counted the dumbbell<br />

connectors in the dumbbells (i.e., they never counted a dumbbell as<br />

three things); adults did. Apparently, a relationship of containment or<br />

connection with a salient object masks the individuation of the less<br />

salient object for preschoolers, but not for adults.<br />

(3015)<br />

Breaking Weber’s Law: Systematic Deviations in Temporal Production<br />

and Discrimination in Pigeons and Rats. FEDERICO SANA-<br />

BRIA, Arizona State University, LEWIS A. BIZO, Southern Cross University,<br />

& PETER R. KILLEEN, Arizona State University—Weber’s<br />

law is frequently purported to be a ubiquitous feature of time perception.<br />

In two experiments, we tested the timing performance of rats and<br />

pigeons, using two timing tasks (temporal discrimination and production)<br />

across a wide range of intervals (from milliseconds to tens<br />

of seconds). <strong>The</strong> performance of both species systematically deviated<br />

from Weber’s law in both timing tasks: Coefficients of performance<br />

variation were relatively high for very short durations and then decreased<br />

to a minimum for a range of intermediate intervals, as is predicted<br />

by generalized Weber’s law; however, they then increased for<br />

longer durations. A model based on a radix counter that allows for lost<br />

pacemaker counts from an internal clock (Killeen & Taylor, 2000;<br />

Killeen & Weiss, 1987) accommodated the U-shaped pattern.<br />

(3016)<br />

Psychological Time Distortions in Judging Minute-Long Intervals<br />

Retrospectively. SIMON GRONDIN & MARILYN PLOURDE, Université<br />

Laval—Fifty participants were asked to perform five different<br />

cognitive tasks lasting 2, 3.5, 5, 6.5, and 8 min, respectively. After<br />

completing the series of tasks, they were asked to estimate retrospectively<br />

the duration of each one. Psychophysical analyses linking psychological<br />

time to physical time revealed that the value of the slope,<br />

when expressed with the power law, was about .48. <strong>The</strong> coefficient of<br />

variation, or the ratio of variability to estimated time, ranged from .38<br />

(at 2 min) to .28 (at 8 min). A task effect was observed only at Position<br />

1 for estimated time, whereas no position effect was observed for<br />

each task. <strong>The</strong>re was no significant position or task effect for the coefficient<br />

of variation. At the individual level, the results revealed severe<br />

psychological time distortions. For instance, 2 and 8 min were<br />

sometimes estimated as the same duration.<br />

(3017)<br />

Varying Duration and Location of a Tone in Time Production. JULIE<br />

CHAMPAGNE & CLAUDETTE FORTIN, Université Laval—Manipulating<br />

location of signals in time productions lengthens produced intervals<br />

in proportion to the duration for which the signal is expected.<br />

This effect was attributed to attention sharing between the timing and<br />

the monitoring of the source of the signal. <strong>The</strong> present study examines<br />

whether the location effect is influenced by the processing requirements<br />

of the signal. A tone was used in a high- or low-frequency<br />

discrimination task, the tone being either short (10 msec) or long<br />

(150 msec). Participants were tested in two sessions with conditions<br />

of tone duration counterbalanced. <strong>The</strong> location effect was stronger<br />

when participants were tested first in the short-tone condition than<br />

when they were tested first in the long-tone condition. This was explained<br />

by a sampling strategy developed in the first experimental session<br />

that participants maintain during the whole experiment.<br />

(3018)<br />

Remembering the Rate and Duration of Naturalistic Events. MARI-<br />

LYN G. BOLTZ, Haverford College—In everyday behavior, there are<br />

90<br />

many situations that require one to judge an event’s rate and/or total<br />

duration. <strong>The</strong> purpose of the present research is to compare the recognition<br />

memory for these two temporal dimensions. Experiment 1 revealed<br />

that people are able to incidentally learn the inherent rate of<br />

various ecological sounds that vary in their structural characteristics.<br />

Experiment 2 further demonstrated that rate information is both better<br />

remembered and acquired after a fewer number of learning trials<br />

than is event duration. Lastly, Experiment 3 systematically manipulated<br />

participants’ attending to rate, duration, and pitch information<br />

in order to assess whether these three structural dimensions are jointly<br />

or independently encoded into memory. This relationship, in turn, has<br />

both practical and theoretical implications.<br />

(3019)<br />

Assessment of Numerosity by Tamarins. JULIE J. NEIWORTH,<br />

ALISON LEWIS, & MAREN SONSTEGARD, Carleton College—<br />

Tamarins were trained in two different tasks, one involving a discrimination<br />

of a particular number presented simultaneously on a<br />

three-dimensional visual array and one involving a discrimination of<br />

a particular number of sequentially presented items. Tests of generalization<br />

verified relative numerosity assessment in both tasks initially.<br />

With more training of two particular numbers in each task, generalization<br />

tests showed tamarins subitizing, rather than assessing, relative<br />

numerosity. As Wright, Colombo, and others have found with categorization<br />

tasks, training with multiple items shifted tamarins’<br />

assessment to a more complex method.<br />

(3020)<br />

Scalar Effects in Birds’ Visual Discrimination of Numerosity. JACKY<br />

EMMERTON & JENNIFER RENNER, Purdue University—Several<br />

models of numerical discrimination posit scalar effects—that is, an increase<br />

in the variance of behavioral choices proportional to stimulus<br />

magnitude (numerical value). Pigeons were trained to discriminate<br />

pairs of anchor numerosities (ranging from 2 vs. 7 to 20 vs. 80). <strong>The</strong>y<br />

demonstrated both interpolation and extrapolation of their choices in<br />

tests with novel values. Psychophysical functions were similar within<br />

a given ratio of anchor values when absolute numerosities differed.<br />

Points of subjective equality (PSEs) conformed to geometric means<br />

of the anchor numerosities, and difference limens increased in proportion<br />

to PSEs. Birds’ performance was robust under various control<br />

conditions, including randomizing the array configurations and<br />

array element sizes and equating across arrays for summed brightness<br />

and area. Effects found across a range of numerosities with these visual<br />

arrays were similar to those previously reported across species<br />

when discrimination of number (as well as time) was based on sequential,<br />

nonvisual items.<br />

(3021)<br />

Absolute Timing. JONATHON D. CRYSTAL, University of Georgia—<br />

Independent groups of rats were trained with a 100-sec (n = 14), 24-sec<br />

(n = 7), or 12-sec (n = 7) fixed interval (FI) procedure. <strong>The</strong> postreinforcement<br />

pause (PRP; latency to the first response after food) was<br />

measured. Temporal uncertainty differed between the three conditions<br />

when measured on a relative time scale (i.e., PRP/FI); temporal uncertainty<br />

was approximately the same when measured on an absolute<br />

time scale (i.e., FI � PRP). Timing was optimal (PRP = FI) for the<br />

100- and 12-sec groups. <strong>The</strong> data provide support for the absolute timing<br />

hypothesis (i.e., temporal uncertainty is constant in absolute time,<br />

rather than in relative time).<br />

(3022)<br />

Testing a Basic Assumption of Interval Models of Short-Interval<br />

Timing: Does the Internal Clock Work Like a Stopwatch? J. DEVIN<br />

MCAULEY, LORI CURTINDALE, & KEVIN C. H. PANG, Bowling<br />

Green State University—Interval models of short-interval timing posit<br />

three independent processing components: an internal clock used to<br />

estimate duration, a reference memory used to store information about<br />

duration, and a comparison mechanism used to judge relative dura-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!