29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Papers 48–53 Friday Morning<br />

view that older adults’ reduced source memory causes their false<br />

memories. That is, we observed that even when older and younger<br />

adults showed equivalent source accuracy for all item types, older<br />

adults were still more than twice as likely as their younger counterparts<br />

to claim that they had witnessed events in a video that were only<br />

suggested in a questionnaire. Moreover, older adults were most likely<br />

to commit suggestibility errors when they were most confident about<br />

the correctness of their response, whereas their younger counterparts<br />

committed these errors only when they were uncertain about the accuracy<br />

of their response. <strong>The</strong>se results exactly fit the predictions of a<br />

vivid false recollection hypothesis about the effects of cognitive aging<br />

on episodic memory.<br />

Psycholinguistics<br />

Grand Ballroom West, Friday Morning, 10:20–12:00<br />

Chaired by Sam Glucksberg, Princeton University<br />

10:20–10:35 (48)<br />

Understanding Novel Metaphors: When Comparison Fails. SAM<br />

GLUCKSBERG & CATRINEL HAUGHT, Princeton University—<br />

Bowdle and Gentner recently argued that novel metaphors are invariably<br />

understood as comparisons, whereas conventional ones can be<br />

understood either as comparisons or as categorizations. We show that<br />

whether a metaphor is understood as a comparison or as a categorization<br />

depends on the relative aptness of the two forms. Using a<br />

novel adjectival modification paradigm, we show that novel metaphors,<br />

such as “billboards are advertising warts,” can be understood as categorizations<br />

but are anomalous as comparisons. We also show that<br />

some metaphors and their corresponding similes have distinctly different<br />

interpretations. <strong>The</strong>se findings lead to the conclusion that comparison<br />

theories of metaphor are fundamentally flawed.<br />

10:40–10:55 (49)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Processing of Novel Metaphors by the Two Cerebral Hemispheres.<br />

MIRIAM FAUST & NIRA MASHAL, Bar-Ilan University,<br />

TALMA HENDLER, Wohl Institute for Advanced Imaging, Sourasky<br />

Medical Center, Tel-Aviv, ABRAHAM GOLDSTEIN & YOSSI AR-<br />

ZOUAN, Bar-Ilan University, & MARK JUNG-BEEMAN, Northwestern<br />

University—Some research indicates that the right cerebral<br />

hemisphere (RH) plays a unique role in comprehending the figurative<br />

meaning of metaphors, whereas the results of other studies do not support<br />

the notion of a selective role for the RH in accessing metaphorical<br />

meanings. In the present research, participants were presented<br />

with four types of word pairs—literal, conventional metaphoric, novel<br />

metaphoric, and unrelated—and were asked to decide whether each<br />

expression was meaningful or not. <strong>The</strong> findings of behavioral and<br />

ERPs experiments suggest that the RH contributes significantly to the<br />

processing of novel metaphoric expressions. Furthermore, the results<br />

of an fMRI experiment show that a unique network, consisting of the<br />

right homologue of Wernicke’s area, the left and right premotor areas,<br />

the right and left insula, and Broca’s area, is recruited for the processing<br />

of novel metaphors, but not for the processing of conventional<br />

metaphors.<br />

11:00–11:15 (50)<br />

Determinants of Speed of Processing Effects on Spoken Idiom<br />

Comprehension. CRISTINA CACCIARI & ROBERTO PADOVANI,<br />

University of Modena—Using a cross-modal lexical decision paradigm,<br />

we investigated the extent to which individual speed of processing<br />

(SOP) affected spoken language comprehension in languageunimpaired<br />

participants when the sentence contained a semantically<br />

ambiguous Italian idiom (e.g., “break the ice”) embedded in an idiomrelated<br />

context. To differentiate among sensorimotor speed components,<br />

cognitive speed components, and personality-based components,<br />

we investigated the main factors that might account for the<br />

differences in idiom activation latencies between fast versus slow participants.<br />

<strong>The</strong> participants enrolled in the cross-modal study were pre-<br />

8<br />

sented with a battery of tests assessing (1) the SOP in nonlinguistic<br />

tasks, (2) the ability to inhibit irrelevant information, (3) the role of<br />

verbal working memory components, (4) word recognition, lexical access,<br />

and reading abilities, (5) nonverbal intelligence, and (6) personality<br />

structure and anxiety level. <strong>The</strong> speed with which idiomatic<br />

meanings were activated was modulated by phonological, semantic,<br />

memory-based, and personality components.<br />

11:20–11:35 (51)<br />

Laughter in Bill Clinton’s My Life (2004) Interviews. DANIEL C.<br />

O’CONNELL, Georgetown University, & SABINE KOWAL, Technical<br />

University of Berlin—Two types of laughter of Bill Clinton and his<br />

interviewers—as an overlay of words spoken laughingly and as laughter<br />

of the ha-ha sort—were investigated in 13 media interviews given<br />

after the publication of his memoirs. His laughter was found to be predominantly<br />

an overlay of words spoken laughingly, whereas Hillary<br />

Clinton’s had been found to be largely of the ha-ha type (O’Connell<br />

& Kowal, 2004). <strong>The</strong> presence of interviewers’ laughter disallowed<br />

neutralism on their part. Ha-ha laughter occurred as interruption or<br />

back channeling 30% of the time and, hence, did not necessarily punctuate<br />

speech during pauses at the end of phrases and sentences (Provine,<br />

1993). Laughter did not follow upon “banal comments” (Provine, 2004,<br />

p. 215) nor reflect either “nonseriousness” (Chafe, 2003a, 2003b) or<br />

uncensored spontaneity (Provine, 2004, p. 216). Instead, laughter reflected<br />

the perspective of interviewer and/or interviewee and was used<br />

as a deliberate, sophisticated, and rhetorical device.<br />

11:40–11:55 (52)<br />

Comprehending Stuttered Speech in Different Referential Contexts.<br />

MICHELLE F. LEVINE & MICHAEL F. SCHOBER, New School for<br />

Social Research (read by Michael F. Schober)—How do listeners comprehend<br />

stuttered speech? In an experiment, listeners selected one of<br />

two squares on a computer screen on the basis of recorded instructions<br />

produced by a person who stuttered. <strong>The</strong> instructions consisted of naturally<br />

occurring stutters with repeated initial phonemes (“Pick the<br />

g-g-g-green square”), the same stimuli with stutters excised (“Pick the<br />

green square”) or replaced by pauses of equal length (“Pick the . . .<br />

green square”), and matched fluent instructions by the same speaker.<br />

In general, stutters did not hurt and sometimes speeded comprehension<br />

of the target word, measured from the word’s onset. When the<br />

squares’ colors started with different phonemes (green vs. black), listeners<br />

selected the target more quickly when they heard the stuttered<br />

instruction, as compared with any other version. When colors started<br />

with the same phonemes (green vs. gray), the benefit decreased. As<br />

with some other disfluencies, stutters can actually be informative<br />

about speakers’ intentions, speeding comprehension without loss of<br />

accuracy.<br />

Models of Reasoning<br />

Dominion Ballroom, Friday Morning, 10:20–12:00<br />

Chaired by David J. Bryant, Defence R&D Canada<br />

10:20–10:35 (53)<br />

Heuristic and Bayesian Models for Classification With Uncertain<br />

Cues. DAVID J. BRYANT, Defence R&D Canada—Fast and frugal<br />

heuristics provide potential models for human judgment in a range of<br />

tasks. <strong>The</strong> present research explored whether a fast and frugal heuristic<br />

could successfully model participants’ decision behavior in a simulated<br />

air threat classification task. Participants learned to classify<br />

simulated aircraft, using four probabilistic cues, and then classified<br />

test sets designed to contrast predictions of several heuristics, including<br />

the take-the-best-for-classification (TTB-C) and pros rule, developed<br />

specifically for threat classification. When cues were presented<br />

textually, most participants appeared to use TTB-C or pros rule, but<br />

no participant was observed to respond as predicted by a Bayesian<br />

strategy. In contrast, items presented pictorially resulted in a large proportion<br />

of participants conforming to predictions of the Bayesian

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!