Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Posters 2056–2063 Friday Noon appeared in the absence of a unique luminance transient. We found that the new object benefit was maintained. In subsequent experiments, we investigated why others have not found attentional capture by new objects that lacked a unique transient. (2056) The Parting Shot: Effects of Offsets on Attentional Orienting. SHU- CHIEH WU, NASA Ames Research Center, & ROGER W. REMING- TON, Johns Hopkins University—Theories of attentional capture often ascribe special status to stimuli with abrupt onset transients, attributing this status variously to specialized attentional mechanisms (Theeuwes, 1994; Yantis & Hillstrom, 1994) or activation of the saccade system (Wu & Remington, 2003). However, onset properties apparently do not tell the whole story. It has also been observed that the magnitude and time course of capture and spatial cuing is affected by whether or not the abruptly onset cue offsets prior to the target. This is a curious result that seems disconnected from current accounts that attribute capture to salience, new objects, or top-down contingencies. Here, we report experiments that examine whether the offset effect is attributable to mechanisms of spatial attention, saccadic eye movements, or changes in phasic alertness. (2057) One Plus One Equals One: The Effects of Merging on Object Files. STEPHEN R. MITROFF, Duke University, & BRIAN J. SCHOLL & KAREN WYNN, Yale University—A critical task in visual processing is keeping track of objects as the same persisting individuals over time, and these operations can be assessed in terms of the effects of various manipulations on mid-level object file representations. Here, we explore perhaps the most important principle of object persistence: Objects must maintain a single unified boundary over time (the cohesion principle). We do so by measuring object-specific preview benefits (OSPBs), wherein a preview of information on a specific object speeds the recognition of that information at a later point, when it appears again on the same object. Object files were dramatically affected when two objects smoothly merged into one: The information from only one object survived this cohesion violation to produce an OSPB (whereas OSPBs from both original objects remained in control displays without cohesion violations). These results demonstrate the power of the cohesion principle in the maintenance of mid-level visual representations. (2058) What Do We Learn From Binding Features? Evidence for Multilevel Feature Integration. LORENZA S. COLZATO, Leiden University, ANTONINO RAFFONE, University of Sunderland, & BERNHARD HOMMEL, Leiden University—Four experiments were conducted to investigate the relationship between the binding of visual features (as measured by their aftereffects on subsequent binding) and the learning of feature conjunction probabilities. Both binding and learning effects were obtained, but they did not interact. Interestingly, (shape–color) binding effects disappeared with increasing practice, presumably due to the fact that only one of the features involved was task relevant. However, this instability was observed only for arbitrary combinations of simple geometric features, but not for real objects (colored pictures of a banana and strawberry), where binding effects were strong and practice resistant. Findings are interpreted in a neurocognitive framework that makes a distinction between integration at low-level feature maps, short-term acquisition of frequency-based expectations, and Hebbian learning of object representations. (2059) Attention Degrades and Enhances Visual Temporal Resolution. JEFFERY R. NICOL, SCOTT WATTER, & DAVID I. SHORE, Mc- Master University—Relative to the effects of attention on spatial resolution, a paucity of research has investigated the effect of attention on temporal aspects of perception. In the present experiments, observers made temporal order judgments reporting, in separate exper- 78 iments, either where the first of two stimuli was presented or what it was. Here, we demonstrate both an improvement and a decrement in temporal precision on the where and what tasks, respectively, and argue for qualitatively different effects of attention on the dorsal and ventral visual processing streams. (2060) Effects of Distractor Size on Interference and Negative Priming: Comparison Between Shape and Phoneme Selection Tasks. JUN- ICHI NAGAI, University of the Sacred Heart, Tokyo, & YUKI KOBAYASHI, University of Tokyo—The effects of distractor size on interference and negative priming were investigated to examine the relation between these two phenomena. We compared two tasks (shape vs. phoneme selection) using the same letter stimuli. In Experiment 1, where the participants judged whether the target letter had a rectilinear or a curvilinear shape (shape task), small-sized distractors produced both interference and negative priming. However, in Experiment 2, where the participants judged whether the target letter was a vowel or a consonant (phoneme task), small distractors produced interference, but not negative priming. The results suggest that negative priming depends on the behavioral goal of the current experimental task (see also Tipper, Weaver, & Houghton, 1994), whereas distractor interference reflects several processing levels of the distractor stimuli. (2061) Examining Negative and Perceptual Priming Using a Go/No-Go Same/Different Task. EDDY J. DAVELAAR, University of Maryland, College Park, CHRISTOPH T. WEIDEMANN, Indiana University,& DAVID E. HUBER, University of Maryland, College Park (sponsored by David E. Huber)—In a typical negative priming paradigm, ignored stimuli are difficult to respond to on subsequent trials. In contradiction to this finding, Weidemann, Huber, and Shiffrin (2005) observed a bias to respond with ignored words in a perceptual identification task. Attempting to reconcile these differences, we developed a new paradigm that combines perceptual and response priming within a single experiment. This was achieved with a same/different task (is the target word the same as the cue word) with go/no-go responding (e.g., go only on different trials). Because only some words are responded to on trial n, this allows measurement of response priming on trial n +1. Because determination of same versus different is inherently a perceptual task, RTs include perceptual priming effects. Our results reveal both a general response readiness effect and an item-specific effect with facilitation for primed cues but deficits for primed targets. (2062) Schematic Activation Influences Accuracy and Confidence in Change Blindness Tasks. SHARON L. HANNIGAN, ALAN SEARLE- MAN, EVAN SLATER, & MEREDITH A. PRIDGEN, St. Lawrence University—This study examined the effects of attention on participants’ confidence and overall accuracy in a visual change detection task. Employing a within-subjects design, 46 participants attended to either a featural level (counting the number of objects that contained either of two colors) or a semantic level (generating a name to describe the theme of the objects) when viewing a photographed set of 10–13 semantically related objects. The results indicated that participants were more likely to correctly claim to have noticed a semantically related or unrelated addition of an object than either a color change or no change. A task � type of change interaction showed that participants were more confident about nonsemantically related additions during the theme generation task but were more confident seeing color changes during the color count task. When asked to identify the specific change observed, participants were most accurate in detecting nonsemantically related additions. (2063) Priority Setting Modulates Allocation of Object-Based Attention. GARY C.-W. SHYI, HSING-YI HSIEH, & YUAN-CHI TSENG, National Chung Cheng University—One main source of evidence for

Friday Noon Posters 2064–2070 object-based attention comes from the spatial-cuing paradigm popularized by Egly et al. (1994). An invalidly cued location enjoying attentional benefits may be due to the fact that the spread of attention is faster and/or more efficient within an object than across different objects. However, manipulation of cue validity in the cuing paradigm typically causes the invalidly cued location to have a higher processing priority, which also could account for the same-object benefit (Shomstein & Yantis, 2002, 2004). Here, in three experiments, we examine whether or not priority setting can modulate allocation of object-based attention. The results indicate that when the cued objects enjoyed a relatively high priority, the typical same-object effect was obtained. As the processing priority for the cued object declined, however, the same-object benefit was eliminated. A two-mechanism hypothesis is proposed to provide an integrated account for object-based attention demonstrated by the cuing paradigm. (2064) Attention to Multicolor Patterns: A Binary Mapping Principle. LIQIANG HUANG & HAROLD PASHLER, University of California, San Diego (sponsored by Harold Pashler)—We suggest that people can directly apprehend the spatial arrangement of different colors in a pattern only through the iterative creation of separate “binary maps,” each encoding the spatial arrangement of just a single color (e.g., the red elements or the green elements). To test this, observers judged whether two simultaneously presented four-color figures matched or not. Two conditions were compared: ABBA (e.g., change a red square to blue and change a blue square to red) or ABCD (e.g., change a red square to blue and change a green square to yellow). Binary mapping predicts that ABCD mismatches should be substantially easier to find, because here a mismatch will be discovered in the first binary map checked. This prediction was confirmed. The potential relevance of binary mapping to various theoretical issues in visual attention and to multidimensional data visualization will be pointed out. (2065) Limits of Redundancy Gain and Coactivation in Three-Dimensional Visual Stimuli. SONJA ENGMANN & DENIS COUSINEAU, Université de Montréal (sponsored by Denis Cousineau)—Response times of participants in a visual object recognition task decrease significantly if targets can be distinguished by several redundant attributes. Statistical facilitation models predict such a gain (Townsend & Ashby, 1983). However, coactivation models give a better approximation of the strength of gain (Miller, 1982). Triple redundancy effects have been found with stimuli from different modalities (tactile, visual, and auditory; Diederich, 1992) but are difficult to obtain with purely visual stimuli. The present research examines the influence of varying visual attributes, using three-dimensional redundant stimuli. We study the increase in gain with each added attribute and its dependence on attribute type. We also investigate whether masking influences coactivation as the cause of gain. Results show that redundancy gain of a third attribute depends on the degree of overlap of processing pathways. The perceived gain could be attributed to coactivation. However, the masking effects contradict all present theories of coactivation. • DIVIDED ATTENTION AND AUTOMATIC PROCESSING • (2066) Combined Action Effects Determine the Simon Effect Obtained With Wheel Rotation Responses. DONG-YUAN DEBBIE WANG, University of North Florida, & ROBERT W. PROCTOR & DAVID F. PICK, Purdue University—Four experiments investigated how irrelevant action effects influence response selection in an auditory Simon task, for which stimulus location was irrelevant and responses were wheel rotations. In addition to the action effects of wheel and hand movement produced inherently by turning the wheel, wheel movement also caused left or right movement of a visual cursor in several conditions. Experiments 1–3 showed that the size of the Simon effect decreased when opposing action effects coexisted and suggested that the 79 opposite action effects were combined before the initiation of a motor program. Experiment 4 demonstrated that the size of the Simon effect could be changed gradually by manipulating the period in the task sequence during which the wheel controlled the cursor. A combined action effect model is proposed to account for the size change of the Simon effect when action effects are in opposition. (2067) Stimulus–Response Associations in Dual-Task Stroop. MARK G. VAN SELST, JENNIFER CHENG, & TAO-CHUNG (TIM) WANG, San Jose State University—In a continuing series of experiments, the role of stimulus–response compatibility in dual-task stroop interference is examined. Prior work indicates Stroop interference to be additive across SOA in the PRP paradigm, despite presenting half of the stimuli with each letter individually rotated 180º in the picture plane. This finding holds for verbal, as well as manual, responses and when the response is “red” versus “not red,” rather than “red” versus “green.” In contrast to some expectations, the patterns of data do not support the notion that the “red” versus the “not-red” condition produces qualitatively different processing than the “red” versus “green” condition, even in the manual response condition, to yield the weakest S–R connection. (2068) New Analysis of Stroop Matching Task Calls for Theory Reevaluation. AVISHAI HENIK & LIAT GOLDFARB, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev—In Stroop matching tasks, participants match between an object color and the meaning of a colored color word. Previously, it was concluded that interference between two incongruent representations of the same attribute (ink color) appears prior to the response stage. This conclusion is based on questionable data analysis. We suggest analyzing the data by separating “same” and “different” responses and then analyzing three congruency conditions within the “different” responses: (1) congruent word color–word meaning, (2) congruent word color–object color, and (3) incongruent word color, word meaning, and another object color. This analysis reveals that responding is slowest in the first condition. This pattern of results does not fit with previous conclusions regarding this task but supports the contributions of task conflict and response competition. This analysis has implications for matching tasks other than the Stroop matching task. (2069) Lateralized Readiness Potential Evidence for Parallel Response Selection in Dual Tasks. SCOTT WATTER, JUDITH M. SHEDDEN, & JENNIFER J. HEISZ, McMaster University—Recent work (e.g., Hommel, 1998; Watter & Logan, 2005) shows that in a typical psychological refractory period (PRP) dual-task paradigm, the manual response to a later Task 2 can influence Task 1 responding, suggesting that response information for Task 2 is available prior to completion of Task 1 response selection, violating Pashler’s (1994) response selection bottleneck framework. We employed ERP methods to assess the timing and cross-task influences of response selection processes in a PRP paradigm. Building on critical earlier lateral readiness potential (LRP) information processing work by Osman and Moore (1993) and Gratton et al. (1988), we sought to better quantify critical Task 2 to Task 1 response compatibility effects—previously observed in RT measures and taken as evidence for parallel operation of response selection processes—by investigating LRP latencies and amplitudes for these priming effects. Priming of Task 1 response selection from Task 2 response information was observed, along with LRP morphologies reflecting Task 2 to Task 1 influence of response selection. (2070) The Role of Perceptual Emergent Features in Dual-Task Performance. KIM-PHUONG L. VU, California State University, Long Beach, & ROBERT W. PROCTOR, Purdue University—For dual-task performance with all combinations of compatible and incompatible stimulus–response mappings, performance is better when the map-

Posters 2056–2063 Friday Noon<br />

appeared in the absence of a unique luminance transient. We found<br />

that the new object benefit was maintained. In subsequent experiments,<br />

we investigated why others have not found attentional capture<br />

by new objects that lacked a unique transient.<br />

(2056)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Parting Shot: Effects of Offsets on Attentional Orienting. SHU-<br />

CHIEH WU, NASA Ames Research Center, & ROGER W. REMING-<br />

TON, Johns Hopkins University—<strong>The</strong>ories of attentional capture often<br />

ascribe special status to stimuli with abrupt onset transients, attributing<br />

this status variously to specialized attentional mechanisms (<strong>The</strong>euwes,<br />

1994; Yantis & Hillstrom, 1994) or activation of the saccade system<br />

(Wu & Remington, 2003). However, onset properties apparently do<br />

not tell the whole story. It has also been observed that the magnitude<br />

and time course of capture and spatial cuing is affected by whether or<br />

not the abruptly onset cue offsets prior to the target. This is a curious<br />

result that seems disconnected from current accounts that attribute<br />

capture to salience, new objects, or top-down contingencies. Here, we<br />

report experiments that examine whether the offset effect is attributable<br />

to mechanisms of spatial attention, saccadic eye movements, or<br />

changes in phasic alertness.<br />

(2057)<br />

One Plus One Equals One: <strong>The</strong> Effects of Merging on Object Files.<br />

STEPHEN R. MITROFF, Duke University, & BRIAN J. SCHOLL &<br />

KAREN WYNN, Yale University—A critical task in visual processing<br />

is keeping track of objects as the same persisting individuals over<br />

time, and these operations can be assessed in terms of the effects of<br />

various manipulations on mid-level object file representations. Here,<br />

we explore perhaps the most important principle of object persistence:<br />

Objects must maintain a single unified boundary over time (the cohesion<br />

principle). We do so by measuring object-specific preview benefits<br />

(OSPBs), wherein a preview of information on a specific object<br />

speeds the recognition of that information at a later point, when it appears<br />

again on the same object. Object files were dramatically affected<br />

when two objects smoothly merged into one: <strong>The</strong> information<br />

from only one object survived this cohesion violation to produce an<br />

OSPB (whereas OSPBs from both original objects remained in control<br />

displays without cohesion violations). <strong>The</strong>se results demonstrate<br />

the power of the cohesion principle in the maintenance of mid-level<br />

visual representations.<br />

(2058)<br />

What Do We Learn From Binding Features? Evidence for Multilevel<br />

Feature Integration. LORENZA S. COLZATO, Leiden University,<br />

ANTONINO RAFFONE, University of Sunderland, & BERNHARD<br />

HOMMEL, Leiden University—Four experiments were conducted to<br />

investigate the relationship between the binding of visual features (as<br />

measured by their aftereffects on subsequent binding) and the learning<br />

of feature conjunction probabilities. Both binding and learning effects<br />

were obtained, but they did not interact. Interestingly, (shape–color)<br />

binding effects disappeared with increasing practice, presumably due<br />

to the fact that only one of the features involved was task relevant.<br />

However, this instability was observed only for arbitrary combinations<br />

of simple geometric features, but not for real objects (colored pictures<br />

of a banana and strawberry), where binding effects were strong and<br />

practice resistant. Findings are interpreted in a neurocognitive framework<br />

that makes a distinction between integration at low-level feature<br />

maps, short-term acquisition of frequency-based expectations, and<br />

Hebbian learning of object representations.<br />

(2059)<br />

Attention Degrades and Enhances Visual Temporal Resolution.<br />

JEFFERY R. NICOL, SCOTT WATTER, & DAVID I. SHORE, Mc-<br />

Master University—Relative to the effects of attention on spatial resolution,<br />

a paucity of research has investigated the effect of attention<br />

on temporal aspects of perception. In the present experiments, observers<br />

made temporal order judgments reporting, in separate exper-<br />

78<br />

iments, either where the first of two stimuli was presented or what it<br />

was. Here, we demonstrate both an improvement and a decrement in<br />

temporal precision on the where and what tasks, respectively, and<br />

argue for qualitatively different effects of attention on the dorsal and<br />

ventral visual processing streams.<br />

(2060)<br />

Effects of Distractor Size on Interference and Negative Priming:<br />

Comparison Between Shape and Phoneme Selection Tasks. JUN-<br />

ICHI NAGAI, University of the Sacred Heart, Tokyo, & YUKI<br />

KOBAYASHI, University of Tokyo—<strong>The</strong> effects of distractor size on<br />

interference and negative priming were investigated to examine the relation<br />

between these two phenomena. We compared two tasks (shape<br />

vs. phoneme selection) using the same letter stimuli. In Experiment 1,<br />

where the participants judged whether the target letter had a rectilinear<br />

or a curvilinear shape (shape task), small-sized distractors produced<br />

both interference and negative priming. However, in Experiment<br />

2, where the participants judged whether the target letter was a<br />

vowel or a consonant (phoneme task), small distractors produced interference,<br />

but not negative priming. <strong>The</strong> results suggest that negative<br />

priming depends on the behavioral goal of the current experimental<br />

task (see also Tipper, Weaver, & Houghton, 1994), whereas distractor<br />

interference reflects several processing levels of the distractor stimuli.<br />

(2061)<br />

Examining Negative and Perceptual Priming Using a Go/No-Go<br />

Same/Different Task. EDDY J. DAVELAAR, University of Maryland,<br />

College Park, CHRISTOPH T. WEIDEMANN, Indiana University,&<br />

DAVID E. HUBER, University of Maryland, College Park (sponsored<br />

by David E. Huber)—In a typical negative priming paradigm, ignored<br />

stimuli are difficult to respond to on subsequent trials. In contradiction<br />

to this finding, Weidemann, Huber, and Shiffrin (<strong>2005</strong>) observed<br />

a bias to respond with ignored words in a perceptual identification<br />

task. Attempting to reconcile these differences, we developed a new<br />

paradigm that combines perceptual and response priming within a single<br />

experiment. This was achieved with a same/different task (is the<br />

target word the same as the cue word) with go/no-go responding (e.g.,<br />

go only on different trials). Because only some words are responded<br />

to on trial n, this allows measurement of response priming on trial n +1.<br />

Because determination of same versus different is inherently a perceptual<br />

task, RTs include perceptual priming effects. Our results reveal<br />

both a general response readiness effect and an item-specific effect<br />

with facilitation for primed cues but deficits for primed targets.<br />

(2062)<br />

Schematic Activation Influences Accuracy and Confidence in<br />

Change Blindness Tasks. SHARON L. HANNIGAN, ALAN SEARLE-<br />

MAN, EVAN SLATER, & MEREDITH A. PRIDGEN, St. Lawrence<br />

University—This study examined the effects of attention on participants’<br />

confidence and overall accuracy in a visual change detection<br />

task. Employing a within-subjects design, 46 participants attended to<br />

either a featural level (counting the number of objects that contained<br />

either of two colors) or a semantic level (generating a name to describe<br />

the theme of the objects) when viewing a photographed set of 10–13<br />

semantically related objects. <strong>The</strong> results indicated that participants<br />

were more likely to correctly claim to have noticed a semantically related<br />

or unrelated addition of an object than either a color change or<br />

no change. A task � type of change interaction showed that participants<br />

were more confident about nonsemantically related additions<br />

during the theme generation task but were more confident seeing<br />

color changes during the color count task. When asked to identify the<br />

specific change observed, participants were most accurate in detecting<br />

nonsemantically related additions.<br />

(2063)<br />

Priority Setting Modulates Allocation of Object-Based Attention.<br />

GARY C.-W. SHYI, HSING-YI HSIEH, & YUAN-CHI TSENG, National<br />

Chung Cheng University—One main source of evidence for

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!