29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 2028–2034 Friday Noon<br />

(2028)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Importance of Preexisting Long-Term Memory Representations<br />

in Directed Forgetting. KIRSTEN BURGHARDT & FABRICE<br />

B. PARMENTIER, University of Plymouth—In a directed forgetting task<br />

using verbal stimuli, Oberauer (2001) showed that participants are<br />

slower to reject to-be-forgotten items than new probes. This “intrusion<br />

cost” was attributed to a slowly decaying residual activation of longterm<br />

memory traces corresponding to the to-be-forgotten stimuli. <strong>The</strong><br />

present study tested this assumption by using spatial stimuli for which<br />

participants had no existing memory representations and the same<br />

spatial stimuli when participants had developed transient representations<br />

of these items. <strong>The</strong> results showed that the intrusion cost was<br />

mediated by the existence of stable memory representations for the<br />

stimuli: Participants unfamiliar with the stimuli showed no distinction<br />

between to-be-forgotten and new probes, whereas participants trained<br />

to learn the set of locations showed the intrusion cost. This finding is<br />

discussed with regard to current models of memory.<br />

• FEEDBACK AND MEMORY •<br />

(2029)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Testing Effect: Enhanced Retention or Attenuated Forgetting?<br />

SHANA K. CARPENTER, HAROLD PASHLER, & JOHN T. WIXTED,<br />

University of California, San Diego—<strong>The</strong> present study investigated<br />

the effect of an intervening memory test versus a restudy opportunity<br />

on the rate of forgetting of obscure facts. After encoding 60 facts, participants<br />

completed a cued-recall test with feedback over half of the<br />

facts and received an additional study presentation over the other half.<br />

A final cued-recall test over all of the facts was given after 5 min,<br />

1 day, 2 days, 7 days, 14 days, or 42 days. At each of the six intervals,<br />

the proportion of items retained from test versus restudy conditions<br />

was calculated, and power functions were fit to the data to describe<br />

the rates of forgetting following test versus restudy trials. Results help<br />

to answer the question of whether the typical advantage in retention<br />

for tested over restudied items is due to enhanced retention or attenuated<br />

forgetting for tested items relative to restudied items.<br />

(2030)<br />

Successfully Applying the Testing Effect in a College Classroom.<br />

KAREN S. CLIFTON, DAVID L. TRUMPOWER, & STEVEN P.<br />

MEWALDT, Marshall University—Repeated testing has been found<br />

to improve recall of a list of words and of simulated classroom material<br />

in laboratory settings. <strong>The</strong> present study evaluated the effect of<br />

prior testing on subsequent performance on unit exams in eight introductory<br />

psychology classes across 12 weeks of class. Approximately<br />

halfway through a 4-week unit and near the end of a unit, students<br />

took an online quiz consisting of fill in the blank type multiple<br />

choice questions that were easy or relatively difficult; in a control condition,<br />

subjects read statements with the blanks filled in, or they received<br />

no practice. Both easy and hard quiz items significantly improved<br />

performance on the unit exams, when compared with the<br />

control items.<br />

(2031)<br />

Can Controlled Processes Improve Performance in the Misinformation<br />

Paradigm? JOHN B. BULEVICH, Washington University (sponsored<br />

by Mitchell Sommers)—Misleading postevent information<br />

often produces erroneous recollection of memory for an original event<br />

(Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978). Furthermore, susceptibility to misinformation<br />

is modulated by age of the participant, with older adults<br />

demonstrating poorer memory for the original event after misinformation<br />

than younger adults (Mitchell, Johnson, & Mather, 2002). <strong>The</strong><br />

present study examined whether subjective experience and cognitive<br />

control could reduce age differences in misinformation susceptibility.<br />

In three experiments, participants viewed an original event and received<br />

misinformation. Utilizing a multiple test procedure (Koriat &<br />

Goldsmith, 1996), participants received a forced report test with con-<br />

74<br />

fidence ratings followed by a free report test where they were allowed<br />

to pass. Retrieval demands and performance incentives were manipulated<br />

across experiments. <strong>The</strong> results demonstrate that both older and<br />

younger adults can improve their performance when allowed to pass<br />

and that older adult’s deficits are largely driven by miscalibration of<br />

their subjective experience.<br />

(2032)<br />

Suspicious Minds Reduce the Postidentification Feedback Effect.<br />

JEFFREY S. NEUSCHATZ, University of Alabama, Huntsville,<br />

MICHAEL P. TOGLIA, SUNY, Cortland, CHARLES A. GOODSELL,<br />

DEAH S. LAWSON, & JESSICA K. SWANNER, University of Alabama,<br />

Huntsville, & JOSEPH S. NEUSCHATZ, Roger Williams University—We<br />

examined techniques designed to diminish the postidentification<br />

feedback effect. Participants viewed a video event and then<br />

were asked to identify a suspect from a target-absent photo lineup.<br />

Following their identification, some participants were told that their<br />

selection was correct, whereas others were not given feedback concerning<br />

the accuracy of their identification. Some participants who received<br />

confirming feedback were also given reasons to entertain suspicion<br />

regarding the motives of the lineup administrator, either<br />

immediately (Experiment 1) or after a 1-week retention interval (Experiment<br />

2). Suspicious subjects did not demonstrate confidence inflation<br />

effects typically associated with confirming postidentification<br />

feedback. In Experiment 3, the confidence prophylactic effect was<br />

tested both immediately and after a 1-week delay. <strong>The</strong> impact of the<br />

confidence prophylactic procedure varied with retention interval.<br />

That is, it eliminated the effects of postidentification feedback immediately,<br />

but not after the retention interval. Both theoretical and<br />

practical implications of the results are discussed.<br />

(2033)<br />

Retaining Versus Replacing Incorrect Choices in Multichoice Items:<br />

Implications for Learning. DOMINIC A. SIMON, New Mexico State<br />

University—A feature of multiple-choice test items is that the incorrect<br />

responses may become associated with the question and later may<br />

be wrongly thought to be the correct answer, particularly when endorsed<br />

for items for which the correct answer was not known a priori.<br />

This possibility was explored in an experiment in which participants<br />

(n = 77) saw 60 multiple-choice general information items. <strong>The</strong> questions<br />

were presented in random order over four rounds. If correctly answered<br />

twice, an item was dropped from the pool. Items that were apparently<br />

not known a priori were treated in two ways across subsequent<br />

rounds: <strong>The</strong> original alternatives were retained, or the wrongly endorsed<br />

choice was replaced. Contrary to prediction, replacement/<br />

retention of incorrect alternatives did not affect the probability of correct<br />

responding on a test of all the original items following a filled<br />

delay ( p = .093). Number of presentations was related to test performance<br />

( p < .001).<br />

(2034)<br />

Feedback Benefits Correct Responses Made With Low Confidence.<br />

ANDREW C. BUTLER & HENRY L. ROEDIGER III, Washington<br />

University (sponsored by Jason M. Watson)—<strong>The</strong> level of confidence<br />

in a response has a substantial effect on the perception and use of feedback.<br />

For example, high-confidence incorrect responses lead to longer<br />

feedback study times and tend to be “hyper-corrected” on subsequent<br />

tests. Although most contemporary investigations of verbal learning<br />

focus on the role of feedback as a mechanism for error correction, the<br />

present research sought to demonstrate the importance of feedback on<br />

the retention of correct responses, especially those made with low confidence.<br />

Participants took an initial four-alternative multiple-choice<br />

test on general knowledge facts, indicating their level of confidence<br />

after each response. Feedback was given for half of the responses.<br />

Learning was assessed on a delayed (5 min) cued recall test. <strong>The</strong> results<br />

indicate that feedback provides substantial benefit to the retention<br />

of correct responses, especially those made with low confidence.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!