29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 2014–2020 Friday Noon<br />

(2014)<br />

Representational Momentum: Impervious to Error Feedback?<br />

SUSAN E. RUPPEL & CARMEN N. FLEMING, Francis Marion University,<br />

& TIMOTHY L. HUBBARD, Texas Christian University—<br />

Finke and Freyd (1985; Freyd, 1987) claimed that representational<br />

momentum was impervious to error feedback. However, this claim<br />

was based on presentation of feedback during a limited number of<br />

practice trials and on use of inducing stimuli that were identical across<br />

experimental trials. <strong>The</strong> present experiments examined potential effects<br />

of the amount of feedback, while varying direction and location<br />

of inducing stimuli across trials. Experiment 1 presented the same<br />

limited feedback as that in Finke and Freyd’s study and showed no effect<br />

of feedback on representational momentum. Experiment 2 included<br />

four groups, each of whom received two blocks of trials. Each<br />

group received either (1) no feedback, (2) feedback on the first but<br />

not the second block, (3) feedback on the second but not the first<br />

block, or (4) feedback on both blocks. A consistent effect of feedback<br />

on representational momentum was not observed in Experiment 2. Implications<br />

for theories of representational momentum are discussed.<br />

(2015)<br />

Sharing Eyegaze Is Better Than Speaking in a Time-Critical Consensus<br />

Task. MARK B. NEIDER & XIN CHEN, SUNY, Stony Brook,<br />

CHRISTOPHER A. DICKINSON, University of Delaware, & SUSAN<br />

E. BRENNAN & GREGORY J. ZELINSKY, SUNY, Stony Brook—We<br />

explored collaboration in a 2-person target-detection task where the<br />

first person to find the target had to communicate its location to a<br />

partner. Two partners (A, B) communicated verbally using an intercom<br />

(shared voice, SV) or visually using gaze cursors (shared gaze,<br />

SG). In the SG condition, a yellow ring (gaze cursor) representing A’s<br />

eye position was superimposed over B’s search display, and vice versa.<br />

<strong>The</strong> partners’ task was to attain mutual gaze (consensus) on a sniper<br />

target. <strong>The</strong> target, a single red pixel, appeared randomly in the windows<br />

of buildings in a pseudorealistic city scene. Time to consensus<br />

was faster with SG than with SV, and the interval between first and<br />

second partners fixating the target was shorter as well. <strong>The</strong>se results<br />

suggest that sharing eyegaze can be more efficient than speaking,<br />

when people collaborate on tasks requiring the rapid communication<br />

of spatial information.<br />

(2016)<br />

Shared Representational Processes in Spatial Language and Spatial<br />

Working Memory. JOHN J. LIPINSKI, JOHN P. SPENCER, &<br />

LARISSA K. SAMUELSON, University of Iowa—Research by Crawford<br />

et al. (2000) suggests a strong division between representations<br />

in spatial working memory and spatial language, consistent with a<br />

broader theoretical division between “sensorimotor” and “cognitive”<br />

systems (Bridgeman, 1999; Bridgeman et al., 2000; Brungart et al.,<br />

2000). In three experiments, we provide evidence that spatial language<br />

and spatial working memory rely on shared underlying representational<br />

processes. Experiments 1 and 2 show that response delays yield<br />

analogous effects in a spatial working memory task and when participants<br />

give linguistic “above” ratings. Experiment 3 shows that the introduction<br />

of enhanced perceptual structure along the vertical midline<br />

symmetry axis of the task space reduces the impact of this delay in<br />

both tasks. We propose a new model to account for these effects that<br />

builds on the process-based dynamic field theory of spatial working<br />

memory (Spencer & Schöner, 2003, <strong>2005</strong>).<br />

(2017)<br />

Valence-Based Spatial Categories: <strong>The</strong> Role of Stimulus Distribution<br />

and Orientational Metaphor. L. ELIZABETH CRAWFORD &<br />

JOHN T. DRAKE, University of Richmond—In a study of inductive<br />

spatial category learning, we manipulated the spatial distribution of<br />

valenced stimuli to examine whether people form spatial categories<br />

that capture regions of common stimulus valence, and whether category<br />

learning is supported when the distribution is consistent with the<br />

orientational metaphor good is up. Participants viewed positive and<br />

72<br />

negative stimuli in various locations and then reproduced each location<br />

from memory. Some saw positive stimuli in the top half and negative<br />

stimuli in the bottom half of space (metaphor congruent), some<br />

saw the opposite pattern (metaphor incongruent), and some saw a random<br />

distribution of stimuli. In the clustered conditions, but not in the<br />

random condition, estimates were biased toward the centers of the top<br />

and bottom halves of the screen, indicating that participants categorized<br />

the space into valence-based regions. This category effect was<br />

significantly stronger in the metaphor-congruent than in the metaphorincongruent<br />

condition.<br />

(2018)<br />

Spatial Navigation Response to Social Objects in a Virtual Environment.<br />

MARC M. SEBRECHTS, ANDREAS E. FINKELMEYER, &<br />

CHERYL Y. TREPAGNIER, Catholic University of America—Eight<br />

participants were asked to navigate through a virtual mall in order to<br />

locate objects in stores, and paths and comments were recorded. <strong>The</strong><br />

virtual mall included both social (two persons conversing) and nonsocial<br />

(two similarly spaced but inanimate) objects. Participant comments<br />

indicated that the environment was treated in a realistic manner.<br />

Individuals typically chose paths that represented the least<br />

distance to their objectives, even when this required passing between<br />

two persons. When distance cost for alternative routes was minimized<br />

and simulated speech between virtual figures was made audible, participants<br />

modified their paths to avoid passing between the represented<br />

persons. <strong>The</strong> utility of this approach for assessing social awareness<br />

and responsiveness is discussed.<br />

• RETRIEVAL INHIBITION •<br />

(2019)<br />

Conditions Influencing Retrieval-Induced Forgetting in Episodic<br />

Memory. DAVID P. YELLS, Utah Valley State College, & KENNETH<br />

A. DEFFENBACHER, University of Nebraska, Omaha—Retrievalinduced<br />

forgetting (RIF) occurs when practicing a subset of items<br />

from a study list reduces subsequent retrieval of the remaining items<br />

from that list. We attempted to demonstrate RIF in an episodic memory<br />

paradigm using abstract nouns, several different intermediate<br />

practice procedures, and a final recall task. In Experiment 1, RIF occurred<br />

when the practice task was based on the initial letter of the<br />

study items. In Experiment 2, RIF did not occur when the practice task<br />

was based on the color of the study items. In Experiment 3, RIF did<br />

not occur when the practice task was based on the background color<br />

of the study list. Our results further support our contention that in<br />

episodic memory paradigms, the occurrence of RIF is a function of<br />

the nature of the study items, the type of practice task, and the type of<br />

final retrieval task.<br />

(2020)<br />

Retrieval Induced Facilitation: Manifestation of the Testing Effect on<br />

Previous Untested Materials. JASON C. K. CHAN, KATHLEEN B.<br />

MCDERMOTT, & HENRY L. ROEDIGER III, Washington University—<br />

Three experiments used educationally relevant prose materials to examine<br />

how taking initial tests affects later memory for materials not<br />

initially tested (but related to the materials that were tested). Experiments<br />

1 and 2 showed that taking two initial tests on a subset of the<br />

studied materials enhanced accurate recall performance on the initially<br />

untested materials 24 h later, as compared with conditions in<br />

which no retrieval practice had occurred. Importantly, this facilitative<br />

effect was not seen when subjects were given additional study opportunities<br />

(but not retrieval practice), suggesting a retrieval-specific facilitative<br />

mechanism. Experiment 3 demonstrated this facilitative effect<br />

in a single-day design in which tests followed in immediate<br />

succession. <strong>The</strong>se results are in line with some findings from the educational<br />

literature (Rickards, 1979) and predictions emerging from<br />

associative memory theories (e.g., Collins & Loftus, 1975), but are<br />

opposite to predictions emerging from the retrieval-induced forgetting<br />

literature (Anderson et al., 1994).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!