29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 2001–2007 Friday Noon<br />

POSTER SESSION II<br />

Sheraton Hall, Friday Afternoon, 12:00–1:30<br />

• AUDITORY PROCESSING •<br />

(2001)<br />

An Investigation Into Nonverbal Auditory Stroop. LAUNA C.<br />

LEBOE & TODD A. MONDOR, University of Manitoba—<strong>The</strong> Stroop<br />

effect is defined as the slowed naming of an ink color when it is presented<br />

in the context of an inconsistent color word. This has been<br />

taken as evidence that it is not possible to completely ignore an irrelevant<br />

dimension. Whereas there have been hundreds of investigations<br />

of the Stroop effect in the visual domain, relatively few studies have<br />

been conducted on an analogous auditory Stroop effect. Previous investigations<br />

of auditory Stroop have been based on examining a conflict<br />

between a verbal label and a physical dimension (i.e., the word<br />

high in a low pitch). In contrast, the present project was undertaken<br />

to determine whether an auditory Stroop effect may be observed when<br />

both dimensions of the sound are nonverbal. If so, this may indicate<br />

that an auditory Stroop effect for nonverbal sounds results from the<br />

inability to ignore an irrelevant dimension of a sound.<br />

(2002)<br />

One More Time: Repetition Effects for Auditory What and Where<br />

Responding. BENJAMIN J. DYSON, University of Sussex, & PHILIP<br />

T. QUINLAN, University of York—Although preservation may seem<br />

unappealing, repetition in the environment may facilitate processing<br />

in the face of continuously changing surroundings. Three experiments<br />

are described in which responses to auditory stimuli varying in pitch<br />

(what) and location (where) were analyzed according to various intertrial<br />

contingencies. Experiment 1 examined Garnerian classification;<br />

Experiment 2 examined classification when target features were selected<br />

within a single dimension or across two dimensions; and Experiment 3<br />

compared feature and conjunction processing. Experiment 1 showed<br />

that the relative separability/integrality between location and frequency<br />

was determined by the relationship across trials. Experiment 2<br />

demonstrated how both cases of within- and between-dimensional<br />

processing may be observed from the same (between) condition. Experiment<br />

3 revealed the locus of the bypass rule to be later than the<br />

top-down influence of a blocked design. <strong>The</strong> study of repetition highlights<br />

important constraints in stimulus processing that are often overlooked<br />

by simpler approaches that ignore intertrial contingencies.<br />

(2003)<br />

Perception of Complex Auditory Events: A Modeling Approach to<br />

Understanding the Source–Perception Loop. RICHARD E. PAS-<br />

TORE, JEREMY R. GASTON, JESSE D. FLINT, & MATTHEW J.<br />

SOLOMON, SUNY, Binghamton—When making decisions about complex<br />

acoustic events, human listeners utilize differential weightings of<br />

multiple acoustic properties, although not necessarily in the same<br />

manner or in an optimal fashion. Our research takes a broad approach<br />

to the whole source–perception loop. <strong>The</strong> completed work on posture<br />

contrast in human gait has defined the statistical distribution of<br />

acoustic properties that reflect not only the posture contrast, but also<br />

other source event attributes that may represent noise to the decision<br />

process. <strong>The</strong>se distributions allowed us to develop simple ideal observer<br />

models to predict performance for each walker on the basis of<br />

each of a variety of acoustic properties. <strong>The</strong> comparison of acrosswalker<br />

patterns of performance for individual models and human listeners<br />

allowed us to begin to identify what properties listeners might<br />

have used to make posture decisions. Our present research utilizes<br />

more complex models that attempt to better approximate the complexity<br />

of human perceptual decision making.<br />

(2004)<br />

Making Up for Lost Time: Is Fast Speech Even Harder for Older<br />

Adults With Hearing Loss? SANDRA L. MCCOY & PATRICIA A.<br />

TUN, Brandeis University, L. CLARKE COX, Boston University<br />

70<br />

School of Medicine, & ARTHUR WINGFIELD, Brandeis University—<br />

Older adults have difficulty comprehending fast speech, likely due to<br />

both age-related cognitive and hearing changes. We have shown that<br />

when time is restored to time-compressed speech, older adults can<br />

benefit from this additional processing time, although at faster compression<br />

rates they do not benefit as much as young adults (Wingfield,<br />

Tun, Koh, & Rosen, 1999). <strong>The</strong> present work studied the relative contributions<br />

of aging, hearing acuity, and cognitive abilities to comprehension<br />

of time-compressed speech and the benefit from time restoration.<br />

We used four participant groups: young normal-hearing adults,<br />

young hearing-impaired adults, older normal-hearing adults, and older<br />

hearing-impaired adults. Listeners recalled narrative passages that<br />

were uncompressed, time compressed, or time compressed with restored<br />

time. Results for the four groups showed a different pattern of<br />

performance for time-compressed speech as well as a benefit from<br />

time restoration, demonstrating effects of both age and hearing acuity<br />

on the comprehension of time-compressed speech.<br />

(<strong>2005</strong>)<br />

Mismatch Negativity for Local and Global Auditory Processing:<br />

Is It Lateralized? ALEXANDRA LIST, University of California,<br />

Berkeley, TIMOTHY JUSTUS, University of California/VANCHCS,<br />

SHLOMO BENTIN, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, & LYNN C.<br />

ROBERTSON, Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Martinez, and University<br />

of California, Berkeley—<strong>The</strong> left and right cerebral hemispheres<br />

have been asymmetrically implicated in local and global auditory processing,<br />

respectively. Here, we tested this hypothesis using mismatch<br />

negativity (MMN), an electrophysiological index of auditory change<br />

detection. <strong>The</strong> stimuli were sequences of tones that distinctively allowed<br />

independent manipulation of local and global patterns (Justus &<br />

List, <strong>2005</strong>). Local and global variations occurred over short and long<br />

temporal intervals (300 and 900 msec), respectively. <strong>The</strong> stimuli were<br />

blocked such that either a local or a global repetitive pattern (standard)<br />

was compared with rarely occurring deviant patterns over the same interval.<br />

Importantly, within local standard blocks, global patterns were<br />

continuously changing, whereas within global standard blocks, the<br />

local patterns were continuously changing. Consequently, the elicited<br />

MMN indices could be unequivocally attributed to changes occurring<br />

only at the level of interest (local or global). We examined the MMN<br />

for local and global processing, with specific attention to lateralization.<br />

(2006)<br />

Selective Attention to Spatially Distributed Environmental Sounds.<br />

STEPHEN LAKATOS, Washington State University, ALEXANDER A.<br />

STEVENS, Oregon Health & Science University, & PERRY R. COOK,<br />

Princeton University—Attention to spatially distributed environmental<br />

sounds was examined using a signal processing system that incorporated<br />

head tracking and simulation of first-order reflections. Participants<br />

were cued to a target at one of three simulated locations in<br />

the horizontal plane and determined whether the target occurred at the<br />

cued location during subsequent streams presented at all three locations.<br />

Four conditions were tested: baseline (no head tracking), head<br />

tracking, addition of reflection cues, and orientation of participant’s<br />

head toward the target. Unexpectedly, head movement cues did not improve<br />

detection, and adding reflection cues and orienting toward the<br />

target impaired performance. Detection was best predicted by each<br />

sound’s perceptual “salience,” measured by post hoc sorting and labeling<br />

tasks, and, to a lesser extent, by its spectral content. Results<br />

suggest that attending to a naturalistic sound in a spatially distributed<br />

context may depend less on the richness of available localization cues<br />

than on the attended sound’s perceptual distinctiveness.<br />

(2007)<br />

Differences in Loudness Processing Between Female and Male<br />

Listeners. ELAD SAGI, DeVault Otologic Research Laboratory, IUPUI,<br />

& LISA M. D’ALESSANDRO & KENNETH H. NORWICH, University<br />

of Toronto (sponsored by Kenneth H. Norwich)—Loudness exponents<br />

were compared in 8 female and 7 male participants using pure

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!