29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Thursday Evening Posters 1120–1122<br />

for facilitating text comprehension in college undergraduates. Participants<br />

read texts in one of four instruction conditions: image, paraphrase,<br />

understand (a neutral baseline condition), and count (counting the<br />

number of nouns). Performance on a subsequent comprehension test<br />

reflected a classic levels of processing effect, with deep encoding<br />

(image and paraphrase conditions) leading to higher scores than shallow<br />

encoding (count condition) and the neutral “read to understand”<br />

condition falling somewhere in between. <strong>The</strong>re was, however, no comprehension<br />

advantage for mental imagery instructions over paraphrasing.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results cast doubt on the value of mental imagery as a<br />

strategy for improving text comprehension.<br />

(1120)<br />

Situation Model Updating for Different Narrative Perspectives.<br />

DAVID E. COPELAND & LINDSEY L. OSTERMAN, University of<br />

Southern Mississippi—Studies of narrative comprehension have<br />

shown that readers often adopt the perspective of characters in the<br />

text. This study examined effects that have been previously observed<br />

(e.g., greater accessibility of associated than of dissociated items) as<br />

the perspective of the text was manipulated. Specifically, comparisons<br />

were made between third-person (e.g., he/she) and second-person (i.e.,<br />

you) perspectives. Within- and between-subjects designs revealed that<br />

people are more likely to show an effect in the second-person perspective<br />

when presented with both perspectives. Implications for situation<br />

model theory are considered, particularly for more recent studies<br />

in which event memory has been examined when people are<br />

actively involved in the situation (e.g., in a virtual environment).<br />

(1121)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of Narrative Time Shifts on the Representation of Goal-<br />

Related Information. WILLIAM H. LEVINE, JOEL A. HAGAMAN,<br />

CARI ANNE BOGULSKI, REBECCA R. GREEN, & DORTHIE S.<br />

ORTIGO, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville—Experimental investi-<br />

69<br />

gations of situation model construction have typically focused on single<br />

dimensions (e.g., time). <strong>The</strong> experiments that we conducted examined<br />

the effect of changes in one dimension, time, on the representation<br />

of information from another dimension, intentionality (i.e., character<br />

goals). In the first experiment, participants read vignettes in which a<br />

character’s time-limited goal (e.g., meeting an application deadline) was<br />

or was not completed. Following the goal completion manipulation,<br />

there was a short or long time shift, with the latter moving the narrative<br />

action close to the time by which the goal had to be completed. <strong>The</strong> results<br />

of a recognition probe task suggest that time shifts had a somewhat<br />

paradoxical effect: Goal information became more active after the<br />

long time shifts. A follow-up experiment examined the role of the nature<br />

of goals on this effect. <strong>The</strong> results are discussed with respect to the<br />

construction of multidimensional situation models.<br />

(1122)<br />

Changing Beliefs Based on Information in Fictional Narratives.<br />

MIJA M. VAN DER WEGE, Carleton College, & AMY KURIVCHACK<br />

& DAVID N. RAPP, University of Minnesota—Nonfiction sources (i.e.,<br />

textbooks, documentaries, lectures) are usually designed to convey<br />

facts and concepts. Individuals, though, often learn facts from fiction<br />

sources (see, e.g., Marsh, Meade, & Roediger, 2003), and their existing<br />

beliefs about facts can be influenced by statements made in fictional<br />

narratives (see, e.g., Prentice, Gerrig, & Bailis, 1997). Interestingly,<br />

Prentice et al. found that university students’ beliefs were more<br />

likely to be influenced by reading fictional accounts set at rival institutions<br />

rather than fictional accounts set at home institutions. In their<br />

study showing general persuasion by fiction, however, Wheeler, Green,<br />

and Brock (1999) failed to replicate cross-institution effects. In this<br />

project, we test several explanations of these discrepant results. Our<br />

replication included participants at comparable liberal arts institutions,<br />

as well as at a large state institution. We discuss our results with respect<br />

to general underlying mechanisms of knowledge acquisition.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!