29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Thursday Evening Posters 1106–1112<br />

jects had to choose, between two strings of letters, the one that was<br />

flashed on the screen. Subjects had higher accuracy for words than for<br />

nonwords. <strong>The</strong> parameters of the overlap model associated with the<br />

encoding of letter positions did not change dramatically from words<br />

to nonwords; hence, the word superiority effect might be related to<br />

processes that occur after the positions of the letters are encoded. In<br />

the second experiment, the model accounts for performance in a<br />

forced choice task in which one or both alternatives have repeated letters.<br />

Repeated letters have been a problem for models of encoding of<br />

letter position. <strong>The</strong> overlap model can account for the data in this experiment<br />

by assuming that contiguous repeated letters are encoded as<br />

a unit.<br />

(1106)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of Mask Category on Letter Detection and Identification.<br />

STEVEN J. HAASE, Shippensburg University, GARY D. FISK, Georgia<br />

Southwestern State University, & ERIC SZELENYI & JOSEPH<br />

HARKINS, Shippensburg University—Following up on our study<br />

from last year, we continue testing Snodgrass, Bernat, and Shevrin’s<br />

model of unconscious perception (P&P, 2004). <strong>The</strong>ir model predicts<br />

above-chance identification given chance detection, under certain task<br />

and strategy combinations. We compared detection and identification<br />

of one of four letters (D, J, K, M ). In one experiment, the masks were<br />

numbers (e.g., 2, 5, 8); in the other, the masks were letters (e.g., F, Q,<br />

Z ). Both detection and identification sensitivity were much higher in<br />

the number mask experiment. Identification was accurately predicted<br />

from detection according to signal detection theory, indicating no evidence<br />

for unconscious perception. In partial support of their model,<br />

we found above-chance identification when participants preferred and<br />

used the “look” strategy (M = 0.315, p = .012) in the letter mask experiment,<br />

where overall detection sensitivity was not significantly<br />

above chance. However, the “look” strategy is defined as one involving<br />

conscious perceptual processes.<br />

(1107)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Influence of Multiple Reading on the Missing-Letter Effect<br />

Revisited. JEAN SAINT-AUBIN, Université de Moncton, RAYMOND<br />

M. KLEIN, Dalhousie University, & ANNIE ROY-CHARLAND, Université<br />

de Moncton—When participants search for a target letter while<br />

reading, they make more omissions if the target letter is embedded in<br />

frequent function words than in less frequent content words. According<br />

to the GO model, this occurs because high-frequency function words<br />

are processed faster than low-frequency content words. This hypothesis<br />

was tested in two experiments (n = 24 and n = 72) by increasing<br />

word processing speed through text repetition which should translate<br />

into higher omission rates. Participants either read the text once while<br />

performing the letter search task or read it three times and performed<br />

the letter search task on the third reading. Contrary to the predictions<br />

of the GO model, omission rate did not increase over repetition, and<br />

it even decreased when readers knew from the first reading which letter<br />

would be used for the letter search task on the third reading.<br />

• DISCOURSE PROCESSES •<br />

(1108)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Contributions of Lexico-Semantic and Discourse Influences to<br />

the Resolution of Ambiguous Categorical Anaphors. TALI DITMAN<br />

& PHILLIP J. HOLCOMB, Tufts University, & GINA R. KUPERBERG,<br />

Massachusetts General Hospital—<strong>The</strong> present studies employed eventrelated<br />

potentials (ERPs) to examine the time course for the integration<br />

of lexico-semantic and discourse information—namely, the resolution<br />

of categorical anaphors. Scenarios were constructed to include<br />

three potential antecedents. Anaphors were semantically ambiguous<br />

in that two of the potential antecedents were exemplars of the anaphor.<br />

Final sentences resolved the anaphor with the correct (associatively<br />

related/contextually appropriate), incorrect (associatively related/<br />

contextually inappropriate), or control antecedent (associatively<br />

unrelated/contextually inappropriate). We examined the amplitude of<br />

67<br />

the N400 waveform, which is thought to reflect the ease of semantic<br />

integration, at several points following the anaphor. Results demonstrated<br />

that (1) the N400 is sensitive to the integration of multiple types<br />

of information at the discourse level, (2) readers use lexico-semantic<br />

and discourse-level information in integrating incoming words into a<br />

larger discourse, and (3) following an anaphor, a correct antecedent<br />

is more easily semantically integrated into the preceding context.<br />

(1109)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role of Connectives in Maintaining the Local Coherence of a<br />

Text. ALEXANDRIA E. GUZMÁN, University of New Haven—<strong>The</strong><br />

hypothesis that connectives aid in the maintenance of local coherence<br />

by allowing readers to form expectations regarding the nature of the<br />

unfolding text was tested in four experiments. In Experiment 1, when<br />

readers were asked to continue a story, the presence of a temporal connective<br />

(meanwhile) led them to form specific expectations about the<br />

nature of the continuations. In contrast, the results of a recognition<br />

task in Experiment 2 indicated that readers do not form specific expectations<br />

about the forthcoming text when they encounter the temporal<br />

connective. Finally, data from a reading time task in Experiments<br />

3 and 4 indicated that although connectives do not seem to<br />

allow readers to generate expectations about the unfolding text, they<br />

do aid readers in the integration of the texts they link. <strong>The</strong>se data suggest<br />

that readers often form underspecified, incomplete representations<br />

of a text rather than a detailed analysis of its content.<br />

(1110)<br />

Distance Effects on the Resolution of Inconsistent Anaphors in<br />

Discourse Processing. KATHRYN H. CHACE, JEROME L. MYERS,<br />

& KEITH RAYNER, University of Massachusetts, Amherst (sponsored<br />

by Nancy A. Myers)—Previous research (Cook, 2003) has explored<br />

distance effects on anaphoric resolution across passages with<br />

consistent and inconsistent antecedents. In a self-paced reading task,<br />

Cook found no effects of distance on how long it took to read the line<br />

containing the anaphor or the spillover line. We used eye tracking to<br />

examine these effects further. Thirty-six passages were used in a 3<br />

(distance: close vs. intermediate vs. far) � 2 (anaphoric congruency)<br />

repeated measures design. In agreement with Cook’s results, there<br />

were no main effects or interactions in first-pass measures of the<br />

anaphor and spillover regions. However, when total time and regressions<br />

into the antecedent region were examined, there were both a<br />

main effect of congruency and an interaction in which more time was<br />

spent reading the incongruent close antecedent than the congruent<br />

close antecedent. <strong>The</strong>se results suggest that eye-tracking data are necessary<br />

to get a complete picture when studying anaphoric resolution.<br />

(1111)<br />

Processing Temporal Information in Narratives: Evidence From Eye<br />

Movements. KRISTIN M. WEINGARTNER & JEROME L. MYERS,<br />

University of Massachusetts, Amherst—In one eye movement experiment,<br />

we examined how the temporal contour of narrative events influences<br />

comprehension. On the basis of the findings from self-paced<br />

reading and recognition probe studies (e.g., Zwaan, 1996; Zwaan,<br />

Madden, & Whitten, 2000), there were two main predictions: First,<br />

time adverbials that signal a time shift (after an hour versus after a<br />

moment) will lead to an increased processing load, resulting in longer<br />

reading times on the time adverbial. Second, the duration of an event<br />

will influence its accessibility following a time shift. More specifically,<br />

reading times on anaphoric references to event information will<br />

be longer following a time shift for discontinued events (the student<br />

stopped studying) than for ongoing events (the student was studying)<br />

that typically persist beyond the temporal boundaries imposed by the<br />

time shift. <strong>The</strong> results provide partial support for these predictions and<br />

are discussed in the context of theories of situation model updating.<br />

(1112)<br />

Inference Specificity and Its Effect on the Encoding of Predictive<br />

Inferences. MARK A. CASTEEL, Pennsylvania State University, York—

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!