29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 1099–1105 Thursday Evening<br />

frequency neighbors will be the word’s strongest competitors, and preactivation<br />

of these neighbors will delay the word’s identification.<br />

Using the masked priming paradigm (Forster & Davis, 1984), Segui<br />

and Grainger (1990) reported that, consistent with this prediction, a<br />

higher frequency neighbor prime delayed responses to a lower frequency<br />

target, whereas a lower frequency neighbor prime did not<br />

delay responses to a higher frequency target. In our experiments, using<br />

English stimuli, we found this was true only when the primes and targets<br />

had few neighbors—when the primes and targets had many<br />

neighbors, higher frequency primes delayed responses to lower frequency<br />

targets and lower frequency primes delayed responses to<br />

higher frequency targets. We offer several possible explanations for<br />

these findings and discuss their theoretical implications.<br />

(1099)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of a Word’s Orthographic Neighborhood on Eye Movement<br />

Latencies During Reading. MELISSA CROCKER & CHRISTO-<br />

PHER R. SEARS, University of Calgary, & PAUL D. SIAKALUK,<br />

University of Northern British Columbia—Many studies have examined<br />

how the identification of a word is affected by its orthographic<br />

neighbors. This research is motivated by the fact that, for most models<br />

of word identification, the number of neighbors and the existence<br />

of higher frequency neighbors have important processing implications.<br />

Reports of neighborhood size effects (faster responding to<br />

words with many neighbors) and neighborhood frequency effects<br />

(slower responding to words with higher frequency neighbors) are<br />

consistent with the notion that a word’s neighbors play an important<br />

role in the lexical selection process. But most of this research has used<br />

the lexical decision task—surprisingly little is known of how the identification<br />

of a word is affected by its neighbors during normal silent<br />

reading. In our experiments, participants’ eye movements were monitored<br />

while they read sentences, and the effect of a word’s neighbors on<br />

eye movement latencies (first-fixation and gaze durations) was examined.<br />

(1100)<br />

Components of Word Familiarity: Orthography Controls Eye Movements<br />

During Reading. JESSICA R. NELSON, ERIK D. REICHLE,<br />

& CHARLES A. PERFETTI, University of Pittsburgh—<strong>The</strong> E-Z reader<br />

model of eye movement control (Reichle et al., 2003) posits two<br />

stages of word identification: an early stage (familiarity) that triggers<br />

eye movements to the next word, and a later stage that shifts attention.<br />

We hypothesized that the early stage reflects orthographic processing,<br />

but that the latter stage reflects orthographic, phonological, and semantic<br />

processing. To test these predictions, participants were first<br />

taught the spellings and pronunciations, spellings and meanings, or<br />

pronunciations and meanings of new (unknown) words, and then they<br />

read sentences containing these words while their eye movements<br />

were recorded. <strong>The</strong> results of this eye-tracking experiment supported<br />

our hypothesis: Orthographic knowledge shortened first-fixation durations<br />

on the trained words, but orthographic, phonological, and semantic<br />

knowledge shortened gaze durations. <strong>The</strong>se results are consistent<br />

with cognitive models of eye movement control (e.g., E-Z<br />

reader) and support the hypothesis that orthographic processing<br />

largely determines when the eyes will move during reading.<br />

(1101)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role of Orthographic Neighbors as Parafoveal Previews in<br />

Reading. CARRICK C. WILLIAMS, Mississippi State University,<br />

MANUEL PEREA, Universitat de València, & ALEXANDER POL-<br />

LATSEK & KEITH RAYNER, University of Massachusetts, Amherst—<br />

In two boundary change experiments, we presented parafoveal previews<br />

that were either identical to the target word, a word that was an orthographic<br />

neighbor of the target (e.g., sweet as a preview for sleet),<br />

or an orthographically matched nonword (e.g., speet). In Experiment<br />

1, the low-frequency word in the orthographic neighbor word<br />

pair was the target and the high-frequency word was the preview, and<br />

in Experiment 2, the roles were reversed. In Experiment 1, the highfrequency<br />

orthographic neighbor word preview provided as much ben-<br />

66<br />

efit as the identical preview and greater benefit than the nonword preview,<br />

whereas in Experiment 2, the low-frequency orthographic<br />

neighbor word preview provided no greater benefit than the nonword<br />

preview. <strong>The</strong>se results indicate that the frequency of the preview influences<br />

the extraction of the letter information without setting up significant<br />

competition between the lexical entries of the preview and target.<br />

(1102)<br />

Do Readers Obtain Parafoveal Information Two Words to the Right<br />

of Fixation? BARBARA J. JUHASZ, SARAH J. BROWN, & KEITH<br />

RAYNER, University of Massachusetts, Amherst—Useful information<br />

is obtained one word to the right of fixation during reading (word<br />

n + 1), which aids recognition of the word once fixated. If word n +1<br />

is completely visible in the parafovea, fixation durations once it is fixated<br />

are shorter than when it is masked (see Rayner, 1998). <strong>The</strong> present<br />

experiment addressed whether useful information about a word<br />

two words to the right of fixation (word n + 2) can also be obtained.<br />

Adjective–noun pairs (e.g., simple problem) were embedded in sentences.<br />

Prior to fixating the noun, a preview was provided of the noun<br />

itself (e.g., problem), a different word (e.g., furlong), or random letters<br />

(e.g., gnahfin). This preview was given when the noun was word<br />

n + 1 or word n + 2. Parafoveal preview effects were obtained for word<br />

n + 1 but not for word n + 2. <strong>The</strong>se findings have implications for the<br />

role of attention during reading and for models of eye movements in<br />

reading.<br />

(1103)<br />

Rereading and Eye Movements. CYNTHIA H. NULL, NASA Langley<br />

Research Center, & TIMOTHY J. SLATTERY, University of Massachusetts,<br />

Amherst—Rereading a text has been shown to increase comprehension<br />

of it. In addition, reading rate increases during subsequent<br />

readings. We explored the nature of rereading benefits as well as how<br />

long they may last by having participants read single sentences once a<br />

week for 3 weeks while tracking their eye movements. Experimental<br />

sentences contained a target word controlled for frequency and length.<br />

In Weeks 2 and 3, the target word in a third of the items changed, but<br />

no item changed more than once. Half of the changes kept the sentence<br />

meaning the same, the other half caused the sentence meaning to<br />

change. Target word and sentence reading times in Weeks 2 and 3 indicated<br />

significant rereading benefits for items that were not changed<br />

and for items that did not change meaning. We discuss these results and<br />

their implications for abstractionist and episodic accounts of rereading.<br />

(1104)<br />

RT Distributional Analyses of Priming and Degradation Effects in<br />

Word Recognition. MELVIN J. YAP & DAVID A. BALOTA, Washington<br />

University—Stimulus quality and semantic relatedness interact<br />

robustly in visual word recognition, with larger priming effects observed<br />

for visually degraded words. According to additive factors logic, this<br />

finding suggests that stimulus quality and semantic relatedness influence<br />

a common early process in word recognition. We manipulated<br />

stimulus quality and semantic relatedness in lexical decision and naming,<br />

and replicated the interaction in both tasks. Distributional analyses<br />

revealed that priming effects in the clear condition reflect mainly<br />

distributional shifting, whereas priming effects in the degraded condition<br />

reflect both shifting and skewing. In contrast with the view that<br />

degradation and relatedness primarily influence a common early<br />

stage, these variables appeared to also influence later processes en<br />

route to word recognition. <strong>The</strong> findings are discussed within extant<br />

models of semantic priming and current accounts of the interactive effects<br />

of semantic priming and visual degradation.<br />

(1105)<br />

Effects of Repeated Letters and Lexical Status in the Overlap Model.<br />

PABLO GOMEZ, DePaul University, MANUEL PEREA, Universitat<br />

de València, & ROGER RATCLIFF, Ohio State University—We present<br />

an application of the overlap model to two experiments. In one experiment,<br />

the model accounts for a forced choice task in which sub-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!