29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 1085–1091 Thursday Evening<br />

how switch costs decrease as preparation time increases in both paradigms.<br />

Differences were found in the nature of switch costs in terms<br />

of their rate, asymptote, and gain parameters. Mixed models of the<br />

nonswitch and switch trial data further highlight the underlying differences<br />

in switch costs between paradigms and lend support to the<br />

notion that an executive control component may play a role in switch<br />

costs for predictable but not for unpredictable task switching.<br />

(1085)<br />

Stimulus-Based Priming of Tasks in Task Switching. IRING KOCH<br />

& WOLFGANG PRINZ, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive<br />

and Brain Sciences, & ALAN ALLPORT, University of Oxford—Performance<br />

costs when switching tasks are related to interference. One<br />

determinant of task interference is whether the stimuli have previously<br />

occurred in the context of competing tasks. To explore the role of prior<br />

stimulus–task association in task interference, we used bivalent stimuli<br />

but had subjects practice them with only one of two tasks (“consistent<br />

mapping,” CM), or with both tasks (“varied mapping” of stimuli<br />

to tasks, VM). In alphabet arithmetic tasks, we found greater<br />

performance costs for VM than for CM stimuli. This suggests increased<br />

interference when stimuli are associated with both tasks because<br />

such stimuli prime the competing task. Using numerical judgment<br />

tasks, we had subjects practice only CM stimuli and then, after<br />

practice, reversed the consistent stimulus–task mapping. Mapping reversal<br />

resulted in massive stimulus-priming effects. <strong>The</strong>se stimuluspriming<br />

effects were reduced by cue-based task preparation, suggesting<br />

a precedence of cue–task over stimulus–task associations.<br />

(1086)<br />

Cue-Based Preparation Effects on Switch Costs and the Contents of<br />

Task Set. AGATHA LENARTOWICZ, Princeton University, NICK<br />

YEUNG, Carnegie Mellon University, & JONATHAN D. COHEN,<br />

Princeton University—This study considers both whether cue-based<br />

preparation affects switch costs and the degree to which this process<br />

interacts with task-set contents. Schuch & Koch (2003, JEP:HPP)<br />

have shown that in a no-go paradigm, cue-based preparation preceding<br />

a no-go trial does not affect switch costs, thus suggesting that task<br />

execution is the primary contributor to switch costs. In this study, we<br />

found that both the inclusion of active no-go trials and the nature of<br />

the task sets switched between can affect cue-based preparation. We<br />

tested participants using two variants of a task-switching paradigm:<br />

varying the no-go trials from active to passive, and varying the task<br />

sets from number judgments (i.e., odd/even, greater/less than 5) to semantic<br />

judgments (i.e., living/nonliving, large/small). Passive no-go<br />

trials and semantic judgments resulted in larger switch costs relative<br />

to active no-go trials and number judgments. We show that cue-based<br />

preparation without task execution can affect the magnitude of switch<br />

costs.<br />

(1087)<br />

Shift Costs and Benefits Following Errors and Error Corrections.<br />

MARCO STEINHAUSER & RONALD HÜBNER, University of<br />

Konstanz—When participants alternate randomly between two tasks,<br />

performance is impaired in task shift trials relative to task repetition<br />

trials. We hypothesized that a portion of this shift cost is caused by<br />

the strengthening of task-related associations that took place on the<br />

previous trial when the response was produced. <strong>The</strong>refore, shift effects<br />

following errors and error corrections were investigated in a series<br />

of three experiments, in which a task-shifting paradigm with a<br />

deadline procedure was applied. As predicted by our strengthening account,<br />

shift benefits instead of shift costs were observed following a<br />

specific type of error (Experiment 1). <strong>The</strong> same pattern of results occurred<br />

when participants explicitly indicated the detection of an error<br />

(Experiment 2). In contrast, shift costs were observed when the errors<br />

were corrected (Experiment 3). It was concluded that the shift effects<br />

in task-shifting are due to the strengthening of associations contingent<br />

to the production of an overt response.<br />

64<br />

(1088)<br />

Alternating Between Verbal and Spatial Memory Tasks: Costs and<br />

Benefits. CINDY CHAMBERLAND & KATHERINE GUÉRARD,<br />

Université Laval, FABRICE B. PARMENTIER, University of Plymouth,<br />

ROBERT ROUSSEAU, Defence R&D Canada, & SÉBASTIEN TREM-<br />

BLAY, Université Laval (sponsored by Robert Rousseau)—<strong>The</strong>re is<br />

ample evidence for a performance cost in alternating between tasks<br />

(e.g., Rogers & Monsell, 1995). Most of the research has so far focused<br />

on the impact of switching task set between identification tasks<br />

that share the same source of information and require no memory<br />

load. In the present study, we examined whether the task switch cost<br />

also applies by alternating between verbal and spatial memory tasks.<br />

<strong>The</strong> procedure required the reproduction of a sequence of stimuli: letters<br />

for the verbal serial recall task and spatially distributed dots for<br />

the spatial serial recall task. A recall performance cost was observed<br />

in the spatial task, but a benefit emerged in the verbal task. This asymmetric<br />

pattern is modulated by the memory set size and the number<br />

of alternations. Our findings are informative with regard to the relative<br />

contributions of interference and reconfiguration to performance.<br />

(1089)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of the Cue-to-Stimulus Interval on Transferable Improvement<br />

in Task Switching. MEREDITH E. MINEAR, Washington<br />

University, & PRITI SHAH, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor—<br />

Earlier work has demonstrated that task-switching performance can<br />

improve with practice (Kray & Lindenberger, 2000) and that this improvement<br />

can transfer to a new switching context (Minear et al.,<br />

2002). In this experiment, we examined the extent to which the<br />

amount of time given to prepare for a switch affects the training and<br />

transfer benefits seen. We manipulated the cue-to-stimulus interval<br />

(200 vs. 600 msec) used during training and transfer. With a short interval<br />

(200 msec), individuals who received practice on switching<br />

showed transferable improvement in comparison with a control group.<br />

However, with a long interval (600 msec), in which preparation is hypothesized<br />

to be complete, individuals who received switching practice<br />

did not differ from a control group on a transfer task. This suggests<br />

that transferable improvements seen after practice on switching<br />

tasks relate to the preparatory component of switching but do not affect<br />

the residual switch costs seen after preparation is complete.<br />

(1090)<br />

Effects of Bilingualism and Task Switching on Hemispheric Interaction.<br />

SUZANNE E. WELCOME & CHRISTINE CHIARELLO, University<br />

of California, Riverside—People perform complex tasks more<br />

efficiently when relevant information is presented to separate hemispheres<br />

(the across-hemisphere advantage). In this study, we investigated<br />

whether variation in bilingual language experience affects control of<br />

hemispheric interaction. Monolinguals, bilinguals who frequently interact<br />

with other bilinguals, and bilinguals who primarily interact with<br />

monolinguals performed two divided visual field tasks. Participants<br />

received a less complex digit-matching task and a more complex addition<br />

task with matching items shown in the same or opposite visual<br />

fields. On half of the trials, the task was the same as the previous trial,<br />

and on half the task switched across trials. <strong>The</strong>re were no language<br />

group differences in task-switching cost. However, language group<br />

moderated the effect of task complexity on the across-hemisphere advantage.<br />

Bilingualism may alter how processing is coordinated across<br />

hemispheres.<br />

(1091)<br />

Sequential Dependencies in a Task-Switching Paradigm. ANDREW<br />

P. HANLIN, SUSAN LAGRONE, & DANIEL H. SPIELER, Georgia<br />

Institute of Technology—Traditional response time (RT) experiments<br />

are interested primarily in variation in RT due to experimental manipulations.<br />

Residual variability in RT is often treated as random<br />

“noise.” Recent evidence has been presented that there are sequential<br />

patterns in RTs over the course of an experimental session (Gilden,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!