29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Thursday Evening Posters 1078–1084<br />

ticipants performed a flanker response-competition task including 1, 3,<br />

or 6 items in the target display while holding 0, 5 sequentially ordered,<br />

or 5 randomly ordered digits in working memory. In a replication of previous<br />

work by Lavie and others, increased perceptual load produced increased<br />

interference in the flanker task. However, the effects of working<br />

memory load did not appear to be additive or independent.<br />

(1078)<br />

Task Reconfiguration and Carryover Effects of Switching Between<br />

Stimulus Sets. SHULAN HSIEH & POYUI CHEN, National Chung<br />

Cheng University—<strong>The</strong> aim of this study was to extend our previous<br />

studies of task switching with response-set paradigms. In this study,<br />

we measured both event-related potentials and lateralized readiness<br />

potentials. In addition, we have used a stimulus-set switching rather<br />

than a response-set switching paradigm. <strong>The</strong> results showed that task<br />

switching may involve both reconfiguration and carryover effects, but<br />

they may take place at different stages in time. Task reconfiguration<br />

specific to task switching may take place before the onset of a<br />

switched task, as reflected on a difference wave between repeat and<br />

switch trials during the response–stimulus intervals; the carryover effect<br />

remains, however, regardless of preparation, and results in prolongation<br />

of response selection processing, as reflected by the stimuluslocked<br />

lateralized readiness potentials.<br />

(1079)<br />

Effects of Oculomotor Inhibition Across the Life Span. BETTINA<br />

OLK, International University Bremen, & ALAN F. KINGSTONE,<br />

University of British Columbia—We investigated the effects of oculomotor<br />

inhibition on saccadic reaction times (SRT) of pro- and antisaccades,<br />

on saccade direction errors, and on the ability to maintain<br />

fixation. <strong>The</strong> degree to which inhibition was placed on the oculomotor<br />

system was varied systematically between conditions. In a noinhibition<br />

condition, prosaccades to a target were required on every<br />

trial. In low–high inhibition conditions, prosaccades and trials on<br />

which fixation was maintained (10%–50% of trials) were randomized.<br />

In low–high inhibition/volitional saccade conditions, an eye movement<br />

was required on every trial, since prosaccade and antisaccade trials<br />

(10%–50% of trials) were randomized. With increasing inhibition,<br />

SRT of prosaccades gradually increased, and the ability to maintain<br />

fixation improved. SRT of antisaccades decreased, as did the amount<br />

of saccade direction errors, reflecting sensitivity of SRT and saccade<br />

errors to oculomotor inhibition. Elderly participants initiated saccades<br />

more slowly and showed more saccade direction errors on antisaccade<br />

trials than did younger participants.<br />

(1080)<br />

Electrophysiological Activity Elicited by Attention-Directing Cues:<br />

Preparatory Visual Activity or Mechanism for Shifting Attention?<br />

JESSICA J. GREEN, JOHN J. MCDONALD, & DANIEL J. WEEKS,<br />

Simon Fraser University—When a central symbolic cue is used to direct<br />

attention to a lateralized location prior to a target, the cue-elicited<br />

event-related potential (ERP) is more positive over the posterior scalp<br />

on the side contralateral to the to-be-attended location. This enhanced<br />

contralateral ERP may reflect preparatory activity in visual cortex or<br />

activity in parietal cortex related to the shifting of attention. To better<br />

isolate the cue-elicited activity of interest, we compared leftwardand<br />

rightward-directing cues with a neutral cue (Experiment 1). We<br />

also sought to determine whether the enhanced contralateral ERP reflects<br />

activity in a retinotopically mapped visual area by comparing<br />

the cue-elicited ERPs for shifts of attention to the upper and lower visual<br />

fields (Experiment 2). <strong>The</strong> results indicated that the enhanced<br />

contralateral ERP arises primarily in occipital cortex, thereby supporting<br />

the notion that it reflects preparatory modulation of neural activity<br />

prior to the appearance of a target.<br />

(1081)<br />

Effects of Background Music on Spatial and Linguistic Processing.<br />

LESLIE A. ANGEL, DONALD J. POLZELLA, & GREG C. ELVERS,<br />

63<br />

University of Dayton—College students were trained according to one<br />

of two standardized task protocols: spatial (mental rotation) or linguistic<br />

(letter classification). Testing consisted of multiple randomized<br />

trials with and without background music (classical piano) at two<br />

levels of task difficulty. Raw data for both tasks were response time<br />

and accuracy. Background music increased the speed of spatial processing<br />

at both difficulty levels, whereas accuracy of processing remained<br />

unaffected. In contrast, background music increased the accuracy<br />

of linguistic processing, whereas speed of processing remained<br />

unaffected. <strong>The</strong>se findings, integrated with those of a previous experiment<br />

from our laboratory, suggest that (1) regardless of an individual’s<br />

predilection, background music can affect aspects of human<br />

performance, and (2) the specific effects, whether enhancing or disrupting,<br />

appear to reflect the similarity between the task demands and<br />

certain objective characteristics of the background music.<br />

(1082)<br />

Effects of Tempo on Rhythmicity of Auditory Attention. W. J.<br />

DOWLING, University of Texas, Dallas, & BARBARA TILLMANN,<br />

CNRS–UMR 5020 Lyon—In 2004, we reported that following a<br />

knowledge-based cue (a melody pointing up or down in pitch, 80%<br />

valid), the convergence of an implicit beat with cue validity led to RT<br />

benefits for identification of targets presented after brief delays. Targets<br />

were fast (8-Hz) or slow (3-Hz) amplitude modulation warbles,<br />

and participants made speeded identification judgments. <strong>The</strong> tempo<br />

of the implicit beat was 72 beats/min. In the present study, we varied<br />

the tempo of the implicit beat between 58 and 102 beats/min. RT benefits<br />

were strongest around 72 beats/min and disappeared gradually<br />

as the tempo was increased. This suggests that if the cue-validity benefits<br />

arise from the entraining of an internal oscillator by the implicit<br />

beat in the stimulus, entrainment occurs most easily when the beat rate<br />

is close to a “comfortable” rate for the perceiving system. Ongoing<br />

experiments are testing slower tempi.<br />

• TASK SWITCHING •<br />

(1083)<br />

Hierarchical Control of Cognitive Processes in Task Switching.<br />

DARRYL W. SCHNEIDER & GORDON D. LOGAN, Vanderbilt University—Hierarchical<br />

control of cognitive processes was studied by investigating<br />

the interaction between higher level sequence processing<br />

and lower level task processing in task switching performance. In four<br />

experiments, subjects performed explicit, memorized task sequences<br />

in the absence of external cues. Switch costs (differences in response<br />

time between task switches and task repetitions) were strongly affected<br />

by sequence initiation costs (slower performance for the first<br />

serial position of a task sequence), to the extent that negative switch<br />

costs were observed for some task sequences. Sequence initiation<br />

costs were found to be sensitive to sequence complexity (as indexed<br />

by the number of within-sequence task switches), preparation time,<br />

and the type of sequence transition (sequence repetition or sequence<br />

switch). <strong>The</strong>se findings suggest that task performance is guided by hierarchical<br />

control structures and that higher level sequence processing<br />

dominates lower level task processing.<br />

(1084)<br />

Mapping the Time Course Function of Preparation for Predictable<br />

and Unpredictable Task Switching Paradigms: A Meta-Analysis.<br />

CHRISTINA V. WASYLYSHYN, MARTIN J. SLIWINSKI, & PAUL<br />

VERHAEGHEN, Syracuse University (sponsored by Paul Verhaeghen)—<br />

A meta-analysis of 71 studies (with 141 independent participant groups)<br />

was conducted to estimate the time course function of preparation in<br />

unpredictable (explicit task cuing) and predictable (alternate runs)<br />

task switching paradigms. Latency (nonswitch RT, switch RT, and<br />

switch costs) served as the dependent variable. Reduction of the<br />

switch cost with advance knowledge is commonly thought to reflect<br />

executive control processes—that is, the intentional preparation for a<br />

task switch. A negative exponential function was modeled to describe

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!