29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 1043–1049 Thursday Evening<br />

ent for categorized and DRM lists, regardless of priming; and (3) false<br />

memories were suppressed for pictures. As with the adults, children’s<br />

false memories are based on interitem associative connections, not<br />

across-item thematic relations, but their veridical memories depend<br />

on both. This dissociation is inconsistent with models that rely on thematic<br />

relations to explain the DRM illusion but is consistent with<br />

knowledge- and resource-based models of memory development.<br />

(1043)<br />

More Detailed Real Memories Can Give Rise to More Detailed False<br />

Memories. KEITH B. LYLE & MARCIA K. JOHNSON, Yale University—When<br />

memory for imagined events is tested, feature information<br />

from similar perceived events may be inadvertently reactivated and<br />

misattributed to the imagined events, producing false memories that<br />

contain actually perceived details (Lyle & Johnson, in press). Here, we<br />

examined the content of false memories as a function of opportunity<br />

to bind together features (shape and location) of perceived events.<br />

Drawings of objects were randomly presented in four peripheral locations<br />

for either 1 or 4 sec. In a center location, participants imagined<br />

object drawings, half of which (e.g., a lollipop) had shape features<br />

similar to those in a real drawing (e.g., a magnifying glass).<br />

Location memory for drawings was greater in the 4- than in the 1-sec<br />

condition. When falsely remembered as seen, imagined objects were<br />

systematically attributed to the location of similar drawings in the<br />

4-sec condition only. Thus, feature binding increases the variety of<br />

perceived detail in both real and false memories.<br />

(1044)<br />

Context Reinstatement and False Recall in the DRM Paradigm.<br />

KERRI A. GOODWIN, KRISTIN MEHR, CAROLINE NORRIS,<br />

BRIANNA RYCYK, & KRISTEN CESIRO, Loyola College in Maryland—Goodwin<br />

reported that when participants engaged in elaborative<br />

encoding in the DRM paradigm, they were more likely to reinstate<br />

their original encoding strategy then when they were engaged in<br />

rote rehearsal. <strong>The</strong> present study experimentally investigated the effects<br />

of retrieval instructions that either matched or mismatched encoding<br />

conditions. Participants were randomly assigned to study lists<br />

using either elaborative encoding or rote rehearsal. At recall, participants<br />

re-created their original encoding for half of the lists and performed<br />

a mismatched retrieval strategy for the other half. Preliminary<br />

results indicate a crossover interaction for false recall in which participants<br />

engaged in elaboration showed more false recall when encoding<br />

and retrieval conditions matched than when they mismatched,<br />

and in which those engaged in rote rehearsal showed the opposite pattern.<br />

Results are discussed in terms of source monitoring and attribution<br />

memory processes.<br />

• EYEWITNESS MEMORY •<br />

(1045)<br />

Distinctiveness of Encoding Determines Susceptibility to False<br />

Memories. HEATHER R. COLLINS & RICHARD E. MAYER, University<br />

of California, Santa Barbara—<strong>The</strong> distinctiveness heuristic<br />

suggests that participants use an expectancy of distinctive information<br />

at retrieval to reduce false memories. We hypothesize that distinctiveness<br />

can also affect false memories for an event. At encoding, participants<br />

heard a verbal narration (nondistinct) or saw a slideshow<br />

with narration (distinct) about a home burglary. During manipulation,<br />

participants received false information presented by a narration (nondistinct)<br />

or a slideshow with narration (distinct). Conditions were based on<br />

information presented during encoding and manipulation: distinct/<br />

distinct, distinct/nondistinct, nondistinct/distinct, and nondistinct/<br />

nondistinct. Although participants in all conditions exhibited false<br />

memories, those who encoded information in a distinctive manner had<br />

fewer false memories than did those who encoded nondistinct information.<br />

Presenting distinct information during manipulation did not increase<br />

false memories. We observed an interaction showing increased<br />

remember judgments for conditions with fewer false alarms (distinct<br />

58<br />

encoding) and decreased remember judgments for conditions with<br />

more false alarms (nondistinct encoding). Encoding can thus impact<br />

false memories more than manipulation.<br />

(1046)<br />

Eyewitness Memory for Own- and Other-Race Faces: <strong>The</strong> Mitigating<br />

Effects of Sequential Presentation and Conservative Instructions.<br />

JESSICA L. MARCON & CHRISTIAN A. MEISSNER, University<br />

of Texas, El Paso, & TARA L. MITCHELL, Lock Haven University—<br />

Meissner et al. (in press) recently introduced a novel lineup recognition<br />

paradigm in which participants encode a series of faces and are<br />

later tested via a series of six-person lineup arrays in which one of the<br />

targets is either present or absent. This paradigm allows researchers<br />

to estimate signal detection parameters while examining phenomena<br />

relevant to application in eyewitness memory. <strong>The</strong> present study extends<br />

this research by using this lineup recognition paradigm to investigate<br />

the cross-race effect in memory for faces (Meissner &<br />

Brigham, 2001). Hispanic participants were asked to encode a series<br />

of black and Hispanic faces and subsequently to identify these faces<br />

from a series of lineup arrays. <strong>The</strong> present study examined whether<br />

variation in lineup presentation method (simultaneous vs. sequential)<br />

and lineup instructions (standard vs. conservative criterion) might mitigate<br />

the influence of the cross-race effect in eyewitness identification.<br />

Results are discussed for their theoretical and applied implications.<br />

(1047)<br />

Creating and Reducing Memory Intrusions From an Unrelated<br />

Event That Precedes a Witnessed Crime. PATRICK O. DOLAN &<br />

VERONICA ZAK, Drew University—<strong>The</strong> eyewitness memory literature<br />

has repeatedly demonstrated intruded memories from postevent<br />

misleading information. Following Lindsay and colleagues (e.g.,<br />

Lindsay et al., 2004), we investigated the likelihood of eyewitness<br />

memory intrusions from an unrelated event that preceded the witnessed<br />

crime. Participants first listened to a narrative describing a<br />

school field trip to a palace that included details of items inside the<br />

palace (e.g., swords on the wall). After 30 min of filler activities, participants<br />

viewed a video clip of a museum robbery that included views<br />

of other museum artifacts (e.g., urn on a pedestal). Results from a surprise<br />

recognition memory test suggest that participants falsely remembered<br />

items from the palace tour as being seen in the robbery<br />

video. However, participants who were given either a strict warning<br />

before the test or a source-monitoring test had significantly fewer<br />

extra-event intrusions. <strong>The</strong>se results are discussed in light of the<br />

source monitoring framework and their practical implications.<br />

(1048)<br />

Photograph Review and False Memory for Recent Events. QUIN M.<br />

CHROBAK & MARIA S. ZARAGOZA, Kent State University—<strong>The</strong><br />

influence of photograph review on the development of false memories<br />

for confabulated events was investigated. After viewing a movie<br />

clip, participants were forced to confabulate answers to false-event<br />

questions. Half of the participants were shown pictures that corresponded<br />

to either the characters or events being referred to in each<br />

question (photo group) and half were not (no-photo group). One week<br />

later, participants had developed false memories for confabulated responses.<br />

In addition, participants in the photo group were more likely<br />

to develop false memories in the absence of confirmatory feedback<br />

than were participants in the no-photo group. <strong>The</strong> results extend those<br />

of Lindsay, Hagen, Read, Wade, and Garry (2004) by showing that<br />

photograph review can facilitate the development of false memories<br />

for recent events as well as for those from the distant past.<br />

(1049)<br />

Creating False Memories in a Group Setting at a College Dormitory<br />

Hall Reunion. JOANNA L. SMUTZLER, MARGARET L. CHARLE-<br />

ROY, SEYEDETH S. HASHEMINEJAD, RYAN M. JONES, & TERRY<br />

R. GREENE, Franklin & Marshall College—College juniors attended<br />

reunions of their freshmen dormitory halls and were told they would

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!