29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Sunday Morning Papers 314–321<br />

sitivity of priming to form overlap in semantic categorization. For instance,<br />

vapary is an effective prime for CANARY in an animal categorization<br />

task but not in lexical decision.<br />

11:20–11:35 (314)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Fate of the Loser in the Competition of Real and Illusory<br />

Words. MICHAEL NIEDEGGEN & MARTIN HEIL, Heinrich Heine<br />

University, & CATHERINE L. HARRIS, Boston University (read by<br />

Catherine L. Harris)—“Winner-take-all” networks are an important<br />

theoretical construct in diverse fields. An implication of winner-takeall<br />

networks is that a word which wins the competition for selection<br />

receives maximal activation and will be consciously perceived, even<br />

when selection was incorrect (i.e., when the winner out-competes the<br />

stimulus present in the input). In the current study, competition was<br />

induced between a physically presented (“real”) word and a selfconstructed<br />

(“illusory”) word (Harris & Morris, 2001; Morris & Harris,<br />

1999). Semantic activation was probed by recording event-related<br />

brain potentials (ERPs). Only words reported by participants triggered<br />

a spread of activation in the semantic network. Nonreported words<br />

failed to prime downstream targets. Both effects were independent of<br />

whether the potential primes were “real” or “illusory.” Findings indicate<br />

that the neural “winner-take-all” network governing visual attention<br />

extends to the processing of lexical units.<br />

11:40–11:55 (315)<br />

Effects of T1 Word and Orthographic Neighbor Frequency on the<br />

Attentional Blink. JENNIFER S. BURT, SAMANTHA HOWARD,<br />

& EMMALINE FALCONER, University of Queensland—In rapid serial<br />

visual presentation (RSVP), identification of a target (T2) is<br />

poorer when another target (T1) has been presented several items earlier.<br />

This attentional blink (AB) increases in severity as the difficulty<br />

of identifying T1 increases. In the present experiments, T1 and T2<br />

were words, distractors were pronounceable nonwords, and the frequency<br />

and orthographic-neighbor frequency of T1 were varied. In<br />

Experiments 1 and 2, the AB was more severe for a low-frequency T1<br />

than for a high-frequency T1. In Experiment 2, when T1 was a highfrequency<br />

word, there was a predicted inhibitory effect of highfrequency<br />

neighbors on the identification of T1 and T2. Effects of<br />

neighbor frequency for a low-frequency T1 were less consistent, with<br />

a predicted facilitatory effect observed for T2 identification only at<br />

lag 2, and not for T1 identification. <strong>The</strong> AB paradigm is sensitive to<br />

lexical variables but different effects may occur for word identification<br />

and word memory.<br />

Animal Cognition<br />

Dominion Ballroom, Sunday Morning, 10:20–12:00<br />

Chaired by William A. Roberts, University of Western Ontario<br />

10:20–10:35 (316)<br />

Studies of Bystander Apathy in Dogs. KRISTA MACPHERSON &<br />

WILLIAM A. ROBERTS, University of Western Ontario (read by<br />

William A. Roberts)—<strong>The</strong> question of whether dogs recognize an<br />

emergency and understand the need to seek help from a bystander was<br />

tested in two experiments. Dogs’ owners feigned an emergency in<br />

which either the owner had a heart attack or was pinned under a fallen<br />

book case and calling for help. Dogs’ behavior was taped and scored<br />

to find out if they would go to an available bystander for aid. In no case<br />

did a dog solicit help from a bystander. It was concluded that dogs did<br />

not understand the nature of the emergency or the need to obtain help.<br />

10:40–10:55 (317)<br />

Remembrance of Places You Passed: Social Spatial Memories in<br />

Rats. ROBERT F. FARLEY & MICHAEL F. BROWN, Villanova University<br />

(read by Michael F. Brown)—Pairs of rats were tested in an<br />

eight-arm radial maze. In one experiment, the two rats made choices<br />

from among the eight maze arms. In a second experiment, the subject<br />

rat first observed the other rat visit four maze arms and was then al-<br />

49<br />

lowed to choose from among the eight maze arms. In both experiments,<br />

there was a tendency to avoid choosing maze arms that had<br />

been previously visited by the other rat. This indicates that the spatial<br />

location of choices by a foraging partner, like those of choices made<br />

earlier by the subject, are coded in spatial working memory.<br />

11:00–11:15 (318)<br />

Rats Can Form a Simple Cognitive Map. REBECCA A. SINGER &<br />

THOMAS R. ZENTALL, University of Kentucky (read by Thomas R.<br />

Zentall)—Evidence for the formation of a cognitive map requires that<br />

a familiar goal be reached by a novel path in the absence of available<br />

goal-directing landmarks. Rats were trained on a three-arm maze with<br />

distinctive arms to find food in two of the arms (the middle arm and<br />

one side arm). <strong>The</strong>y were then tested with both side arms blocked and<br />

novel paths available between the center and side goal boxes. On the<br />

first test trial, rats chose the novel path that led to the side goal box in<br />

which they had found food in training significantly above chance<br />

(75% of the time). In a second experiment, when the distinctive cues<br />

were removed from the arms, no consistent preference was found<br />

(49% choice). Thus, it does not appear that the rats were using a vector<br />

and distance algorithm (path integration or dead reckoning) to find<br />

the goal.<br />

11:20–11:35 (319)<br />

Rats’ Working Memory for Objects Based on Configuration and<br />

Location Stability. VARAKINI PARAMESWARAN & JEROME<br />

COHEN, University of Windsor (read by Jerome Cohen)—Four rats<br />

received three baited objects in the study segment of a trial and then<br />

a fourth baited “missing” object with the other three nonbaited objects<br />

in the test segment of the trial. Rats were more accurate in finding the<br />

missing object when it differed from the other three objects than when<br />

it was similar to them (same) and when the objects remained in the<br />

same spatial location in the square foraging arena between segments.<br />

Increasing the intersegment delay from 1 min to 10 min reduced accuracy<br />

to chance only when the spatial location of the objects was also<br />

changed (Experiment 1). Varying the configuration of the different objects<br />

between segments disrupted test segment search accuracy more<br />

than did merely rotating their configurations (Experiment 2). <strong>The</strong> theoretical<br />

implications of these findings are discussed on the basis of<br />

path integration and landmark stability (Biegler; 2000, Biegler &<br />

Morris, 1996).<br />

11:40–11:55 (320)<br />

What–Where–When Memory in Pigeons. SHANNON I. SKOV-<br />

RACKETTE, NOAM MILLER, & SARA J. SHETTLEWORTH, University<br />

of Toronto (read by Sara J. Shettleworth)—An animal that encodes<br />

the identity, location, and time of an event should respond<br />

correctly to any of the questions “What was it?” “Where was it?” and<br />

“When was it?” when it cannot anticipate which question will be<br />

asked. We describe a novel approach to testing animal “episodic-like”<br />

memory based on this assumption. Pigeons were trained in separate,<br />

interleaved sessions to match the identity of a 3-sec sample on a<br />

touchscreen, to match its location, and to report on the length of the<br />

retention interval (2 or 6 sec). When these what, where, and when trials<br />

were then mixed randomly within sessions, birds were correct on<br />

more than 80% of trials. In order to test whether the three features<br />

were bound together in memory or encoded independently, we gave<br />

birds two different tests in succession after each sample. Binding of<br />

what and where was tested in a different way in a second experiment.<br />

Priming in Memory Tasks<br />

Civic Ballroom, Sunday Morning, 10:40–12:00<br />

Chaired by Eva Dreikurs Ferguson<br />

Southern Illinois University, Edwardsville<br />

10:40–10:55 (321)<br />

Priming, Social Category Content, and Word Repetition Effects.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!