29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Sunday Morning Papers 286–292<br />

enon. We posit that the blink curve is a hallmark of limitations in the<br />

temporal resolution of the binding of working memory tokens to types.<br />

In our model, lag 1 sparing results from a temporal window approximately<br />

150 msec in length during which multiple items can be bound<br />

to the same token. Recovery of the blink occurs when a second token<br />

becomes available for binding. Here, we describe a reduced model that<br />

captures the dynamics of working memory consolidation inherent in our<br />

simultaneous-type/serial-token model. This modeling work is accompanied<br />

by experimental work that corroborates our account of sparing<br />

as a failure of working memory to assign unique tokens to two targets.<br />

9:20–9:35 (286)<br />

Suppression of Attention at Previously Attended Locations: Evidence<br />

From Human Electrophysiology. JOHN J. MCDONALD, CLAYTON<br />

HICKEY, JESSICA J. GREEN, & JENNIFER C. WHITMAN, Simon<br />

Fraser University—Inhibition of return (IOR) is the term used to refer<br />

to the delay in responding to objects that appear at recently stimulated<br />

locations. IOR may reflect a difficulty in deploying attention to recently<br />

attended locations, but most evidence has pointed to inhibition in response<br />

processes. Here, a component of the event-related potential that<br />

is known to reflect the deployment of attention—the N2pc—was examined<br />

in order to determine whether IOR reflects an inhibition of attentional<br />

processes. If IOR does reflect such inhibition, the N2pc should<br />

be reduced or delayed for targets appearing at recently attended locations.<br />

If IOR is unrelated to attention, then the N2pc should be the same<br />

for targets appearing at recently attended and unattended locations. IOR<br />

was associated with a reduced N2pc, which shows that people are slow<br />

to respond to objects at recently attended locations because subsequent<br />

attentional processes at those locations are suppressed.<br />

9:40–9:55 (287)<br />

Correlated Conjunction Effects Under Selective Attention. J. TOBY<br />

MORDKOFF & ROSE HALTERMAN, Pennsylvania State University—<br />

When performing a selective-attention task, subjects are affected by<br />

to-be-ignored items that are associated with responses by the instructions<br />

(the standard flanker effect) and by to-be-ignored items that are<br />

correlated with certain responses (the correlated flanker effect) . . . at<br />

least when the items are defined in terms of just shape or just color.<br />

We extended this work by examining situations involving shape–color<br />

conjunctions and found no standard flanker effect, as would be predicted<br />

by many models, but a strong correlated flanker effect. <strong>The</strong>se data will<br />

be discussed in terms of a multipath model of visual perception.<br />

Language Production<br />

Civic Ballroom, Sunday Morning, 8:00–10:20<br />

Chaired by Craig P. Speelman, Edith Cowan University<br />

8:00–8:15 (288)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Science of Cognitive Systems: A New Approach to Fractionation.<br />

CRAIG P. SPEELMAN, Edith Cowan University, STEPHAN R. P.<br />

HALLOY, New Zealand Institute for Crop & Food Research, & KIM<br />

KIRSNER, University of Western Australia—<strong>The</strong> science of cognitive<br />

systems depends on inferential procedures designed to identify system<br />

components. Such procedures have been subject to criticism however<br />

(e.g., Dunn & Kirsner, 2003). In this paper, we introduce a new<br />

procedure that involves mathematical analysis of abundance distributions<br />

(Halloy, 1998) or Zipf functions (Zipf, 1945), a close relative.<br />

<strong>The</strong> objective is to determine whether or not a given set of data violates<br />

the expected relationship between log frequency and log rank<br />

and demands treatment as two sets or systems. We tested this relationship<br />

for words and letters in English, and for kanji and radicals in<br />

Japanese, using high-word-frequency data. <strong>The</strong> results were consistent<br />

with the hypothesis that whereas kanji and the radicals constitute<br />

a single system, words and letters constitute distinct systems. <strong>The</strong> utility<br />

of this technique supports an argument proposed by Speelman and<br />

Kirsner (<strong>2005</strong>) regarding the relationship between cognition and<br />

complex systems.<br />

45<br />

8:20–8:35 (289)<br />

Memory Processes in Spontaneous Speaking: Measurement of Pause<br />

and Speech Segment Duration. KIM KIRSNER, University of Western<br />

Australia, KATHRYN HIRD, Curtin University of Technology, & JOHN<br />

DUNN & JONATHAN FOSTER, University of Western Australia—<br />

<strong>The</strong> assumption that memory and communication occupy distinct cognitive<br />

domains has relied in part on the use of static and decontextualized<br />

tasks designed specifically to measure one or other type of<br />

behavior. In this paper, we use new measurement procedures to study<br />

retrieval effects under natural language conditions. <strong>The</strong> procedures<br />

enable separation and characterization of the short-pause, long-pause,<br />

and speech duration distributions in spontaneous speaking (Kirsner,<br />

Dunn, Hird, Parkin, & Clark, 2002). <strong>The</strong> experiment involved the contrast<br />

between story generation and story recall and the results were as<br />

follows: First, retrieval task influenced the means and other fluency<br />

parameters under natural language conditions. Second, the critical<br />

pause and speech duration distributions conform to the log normal, a<br />

pattern that implies interaction among the memory and communication<br />

variables. Third, amnesia influences the pause and speech duration<br />

distributions. <strong>The</strong> results further undermine the argument that<br />

memory and communication should be treated as distinct domains.<br />

8:40–8:55 (290)<br />

Structural Persistence in Anterograde Amnesia: Evidence for Implicit<br />

Learning. VICTOR S. FERREIRA, University of California,<br />

San Diego, & KATHRYN BOCK, NEAL J. COHEN, & MICHAEL P.<br />

WILSON, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign—Speakers tend<br />

to repeat syntactic structures. This structural persistence is tacit, incidental,<br />

and long-lived, suggesting that it is a form of implicit learning.<br />

However, most attested forms of implicit learning involve information<br />

less abstract than syntactic knowledge. We determined<br />

whether structural persistence reflects implicit learning by assessing<br />

structural persistence in 4 patients with anterograde amnesia and 4<br />

matched controls. Speakers (1) repeated passive or active primes and<br />

dative or double-object primes; (2) repeated 0, 1, 6, or 10 neutral sentences;<br />

(3) described pictures that elicited passive or active and dative<br />

or double-object descriptions; and (4) made recognition judgments for<br />

the prime sentences or matched foils. Amnesics had impaired recognition<br />

memory for prime sentences in comparison with controls, but<br />

both groups showed significant and equivalent structural persistence.<br />

This is consistent with structural persistence being a form of implicit<br />

learning and implies that implicit learning accrues even to the abstract<br />

knowledge underlying syntactic production.<br />

9:00–9:15 (291)<br />

Do Speakers and Listeners Observe the Gricean Maxim of Quantity?<br />

PAUL E. ENGELHARDT, Michigan State University, KARL G. D.<br />

BAILEY, Andrews University, & FERNANDA FERREIRA, Michigan<br />

State University (read by Fernanda Ferreira)—According to the Gricean<br />

maxim of quantity, speakers provide as much information as is required<br />

for referent identification and no more. Listeners should therefore<br />

expect unambiguous but concise descriptions. In three experiments,<br />

we examined the extent to which naive participants are<br />

sensitive to this principle of communication. <strong>The</strong> first experiment was<br />

a production experiment that demonstrated that listeners overdescribe<br />

in almost one-third of their utterances. <strong>The</strong> second experiment showed<br />

that listeners do not judge overdescriptions to be any worse than concise<br />

expressions. <strong>The</strong> third experiment used the visual world paradigm<br />

to measure listeners’ moment-by-moment interpretations of overdescribed<br />

utterances. This last experiment revealed that overdescriptions<br />

trigger eye movements that suggest confusion. <strong>The</strong> results provide<br />

support for the use of a simple heuristic such as minimal attachment<br />

or argument saturation to create an initial parse. We conclude that, in<br />

general, people are only moderately Gricean.<br />

9:20–9:35 (292)<br />

Effects of Phoneme Repetition in the Spoken Production of Short<br />

Phrases. MARKUS F. DAMIAN, University of Bristol—<strong>The</strong> effects

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!