29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Saturday Afternoon Papers 235–241<br />

stronger than others. We hypothesized that the strength of memory<br />

connections would be influenced by the age of acquisition of individual<br />

words. L1 words learned early in life were predicted to have stronger<br />

connections to conceptual information than do L1 words learned later in<br />

life. Evidence for this hypothesis was obtained in an experiment with<br />

36 highly fluent Spanish–English bilinguals for whom Spanish was a<br />

first language. Participants carried out a translation task in which they<br />

translated L1 words into the second language (L2) and L2 words into<br />

the L1 under conditions in which the words were presented in random<br />

order or blocked by semantic category (cf. Kroll & Stewart, 1994).<br />

<strong>The</strong> results have implications for models of bilingual word memory.<br />

3:10–3:25 (235)<br />

Ubiquitous Control of Lexical and Nonlexical Pathways in Reading<br />

Aloud. MIKE REYNOLDS & DEREK BESNER, University of Waterloo<br />

(read by Derek Besner)—Subjects were asked to read aloud words<br />

and nonwords that appeared one at a time in a predictable sequence.<br />

Control was assessed by measuring the effect of predictable switches<br />

in the lexicality and regularity of the stimuli. <strong>The</strong> results of three experiments<br />

are consistent with the claim that skilled readers can control<br />

the relative contribution from separate lexical and nonlexical<br />

routes when reading aloud.<br />

Masked Priming and Word Recognition<br />

Grand Ballroom Centre, Saturday Afternoon, 4:10–5:30<br />

Chaired by Stephen J. Lupker, University of Western Ontario<br />

4:10–4:25 (236)<br />

Masked Form Priming Effects and the Interactive-Activation Model.<br />

STEPHEN J. LUPKER, University of Western Ontario, COLIN J.<br />

DAVIS, University of Bristol, & JASON R. PERRY, University of Western<br />

Ontario—<strong>The</strong> ability of various versions of the interactive-activation<br />

model to explain masked priming effects was investigated in a series<br />

of experiments. Facilitation effects were obtained from a variety of<br />

form-related nonword primes: standard nonwords (azle–AXLE), partial<br />

nonwords (h#use–HOUSE) and letter pairs (##ai#–TRAIL). Inhibition effects<br />

were obtained from form-related word primes (e.g., able–AXLE)<br />

and from letter primes that, together with the rest of the target word,<br />

create a competing word (e.g., ##ia#–TRAIL). <strong>The</strong> version of the model<br />

that was most successful in simulating these data was one that assumed<br />

that (1) letter activations are reset when the target is presented,<br />

(2) orthographic similarity modulates lexical inhibition such that lexical<br />

units only inhibit other lexical units with which they share letters,<br />

and (3) the lexical units activated by a letter string are more extensive<br />

than just those corresponding to the conventionally defined substitution<br />

neighbors.<br />

4:30–4:45 (237)<br />

Masked Orthographic and Phonological Priming: Evidence From<br />

ERPs. JONATHAN GRAINGER, CNRS & University of Provence, &<br />

KRISTI KIYONAGA & PHILLIP J. HOLCOMB, Tufts University—<br />

Masked priming combined with the recording of event-related brain<br />

potentials (ERPs) was used to plot the time course of orthographic and<br />

phonological processing in visual word recognition. Participants monitored<br />

target words for animal names, and critical (nonanimal) trials<br />

were primed by briefly presented (50-msec) pattern-masked nonwords<br />

that varied in terms of their orthographic and phonological overlap<br />

with target words. <strong>The</strong> earliest evidence for an orthographic effect on<br />

target word processing appeared at around 150 msec post–target onset,<br />

and phonological priming effects were already robust only about<br />

50 msec later. <strong>The</strong>se results fit with the predictions of the bimodal<br />

interactive-activation model, proposed over 10 years ago to account<br />

for a similar time course pattern in behavioral data.<br />

4:50–5:05 (238)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Neural Correlates of Long-Term Morphological Priming. JAY<br />

G. RUECKL, University of Connecticut and Haskins Laboratories,<br />

37<br />

STEPHEN J. FROST & W. EINAR MENCL, Haskins Laboratories,<br />

DANIEL J. YAFFEE, University of Connecticut, REBECCA SANDAK,<br />

Haskins Laboratories, & KENNETH R. PUGH, Haskins Laboratories<br />

and Yale School of Medicine—Event-related functional magnetic resonance<br />

imaging (fMRI) was used to identify the neural correlates of<br />

morphological priming in visual word identification. Subjects performed<br />

a continuous lexical decision task in which prime words preceded<br />

their targets at an average lag of about 10 intervening items. <strong>The</strong><br />

neural response to targets primed by morphologically related words<br />

(marker–MARK) was contrasted with the responses to words primed by<br />

either identity primes (mark–MARK) or orthographically similar words<br />

(market–MARK). Morphological priming was associated with reductions<br />

in activation in a number of cortical regions, including in particular<br />

the occipitotemporal juncture (OT)—an area sometimes referred<br />

to as the “visual word form area.” <strong>The</strong> results are considered in<br />

light of both previous neuroimaging results concerning the neural<br />

basis of word identification and previous behavioral results suggesting<br />

an early, modality-specific basis for morphological priming.<br />

5:10–5:25 (239)<br />

Visual Word Recognition in Hebrew and English: Are the Differences<br />

Qualitative or Quantitative? RAM FROST & HADAS VELAN, Hebrew<br />

University of Jerusalem—Previous studies have reported marked<br />

differences in processing orthographic and morphological information<br />

in Hebrew and English. <strong>The</strong>se differences concern effects of morphological<br />

priming compared with effects of simple orthographic<br />

priming. Are these differences quantitative or qualitative? Do Hebrew<br />

and English represent extreme ends of the same distribution, or do<br />

they differ qualitatively in their lexical organization? We present a series<br />

of experiments that compared performance of Hebrew–English<br />

bilinguals. Our empirical investigation explored effects of masked<br />

morphological and form priming and the impact of letter transposition<br />

on priming as well as on fast reading. Our results lead us to suggest<br />

that in contrast to English, the Hebrew orthographic lexicon is organized<br />

by morphological principles by which words are clustered in<br />

lexical space according to their root morphemes and not according to<br />

contiguous letter units. Thus, lexical architecture for visually presented<br />

words is primarily determined by morphological rather than by<br />

orthographic constraints.<br />

Memory Processes II<br />

Grand Ballroom East, Saturday Afternoon, 3:30–5:30<br />

Chaired by Jeffrey N. Rouder, University of Missouri, Columbia<br />

3:30–3:45 (240)<br />

Selective Influence Within the Process Dissociation Model. JEFFREY<br />

N. ROUDER, University of Missouri, Columbia, JUN LU, American University,<br />

& PAUL L. SPECKMAN, DONGCHU SUN, & RICHARD D.<br />

MOREY, University of Missouri, Columbia—Although the process<br />

dissociation model (Jacoby, 1991) has been extraordinarily successful<br />

in assessing recollection and familiarity, it has a known statistical<br />

shortcoming. In conventional analyses, estimates of familiarity may<br />

be downward biased, making it difficult to assess selective influence<br />

(Curran & Hintzman, 1995). <strong>The</strong> bias comes about if participants with<br />

high recollection also have high familiarity or if items that are more<br />

recollectable are also more familiar. To get accurate estimates, it is<br />

necessary to model covariation of recollection and familiarity across<br />

both participants and items. We do so within a Bayesian hierarchical<br />

model with random effects for participants and items. On the basis of<br />

this improved analysis of empirical data, we show that (1) recollection<br />

and familiarity are uncorrelated across participants; (2) recollection<br />

and familiarity correlate highly across items; and (3) study duration<br />

selectively influences recollection and not familiarity.<br />

3:50–4:05 (241)<br />

Estimating Controlled and Automatic Processes in Free Recall Using<br />

the Process Dissociation Procedure. DAVID P. MCCABE, HENRY L.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!