29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Papers 228–234 Saturday Afternoon<br />

tive to the multidimensional structure of the family tree. I explore this<br />

finding further in three experiments. <strong>The</strong> first tests whether these effects<br />

occur with complex structures for which a familiar prototype<br />

such as a family tree is not available. <strong>The</strong> second looks at comparisons<br />

involving a more complex family tree in which up to four degrees of<br />

lineality are possible. <strong>The</strong> third demonstrates that the results depend<br />

on availability of item-specific information.<br />

2:30–2:45 (228)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effects of Working Memory Capacity and Cognitive Load on<br />

Instructional Design: Subgoals Still a Good Idea. RICHARD<br />

CATRAMBONE, Georgia Institute of Technology—Learners studied<br />

example physics mechanics problems that either did or did not highlight<br />

relevant subgoals. <strong>The</strong>y then solved isomorphs and far-transfer<br />

problems. During training and transfer, participants provided cognitive<br />

load ratings. Transfer performance was superior for participants<br />

who studied examples that highlighted subgoals. Cognitive load ratings<br />

were lower for the subgoal condition during both the training and<br />

problem solving phases. Participants’ working memory capacity affected<br />

performance in the no-subgoal condition but not in the subgoal<br />

condition. This result suggests that examples designed to convey subgoals<br />

can place reduced loads on working memory, so that learners<br />

can more effectively acquire useful problem solving knowledge even<br />

with lower working memory capacity; less effective instructional materials<br />

might require learners to have greater working memory capacity<br />

in order to learn from them.<br />

2:50–3:05 (229)<br />

Symbolic and Perceptual Comparisons With Remembered Instructions.<br />

WILLIAM M. PETRUSIC, Carleton University, SAMUEL<br />

SHAKI, College of Judea and Samaria, & CRAIG LETH-STEENSEN,<br />

Carleton University—Semantic congruity effects (SCEs) were obtained<br />

in each of two experiments, one with symbolic comparisons and the<br />

other with comparisons of visual extents. SCEs were reliably larger<br />

when the instructions indicating the direction of the comparison were<br />

represented by consonant–vowel–consonant (CVC) nonsense syllables<br />

that had been associated with the usual instructions in a preliminary<br />

learning phase of the experiment. <strong>The</strong> enhanced SCE with the<br />

CVC instructions is not readily explained by any of the non–evidence<br />

accrual theories of the SCE (e.g., expectancy, semantic coding, and<br />

reference point). On the other hand, the general class of evidence accrual<br />

views of SCEs, such as those developed in Leth-Steensen and<br />

Marley (2000) and Petrusic (1992), receive considerable empirical<br />

support.<br />

Spelling and Print-to-Sound<br />

Conference Rooms B&C, Saturday Afternoon, 1:30–3:30<br />

Chaired by Rebecca Treiman, Washington University<br />

1:30–1:45 (230)<br />

Long-Distance Anticipatory Effects in Spelling-to-Sound Translation.<br />

REBECCA TREIMAN & BRETT KESSLER, Washington University—Phonological<br />

decoding of written words is often viewed as a<br />

serial process. To test this view, we examined whether college students’<br />

productions reflect sensitivity to English spelling patterns that<br />

require long-distance nonserial processing. Word-initial g is usually<br />

velar (hard), but before e, i, or y, it is coronal (soft) if the root word<br />

is classical or Romance (e.g., giant vs. Germanic give). Students were<br />

more likely to use the coronal pronunciation for nonwords with classical<br />

or Romance spelling patterns (e.g., gepous, gileph) than for those<br />

with native Germanic patterns (e.g., gepness, gileff ). This tendency<br />

was found also for rare words, and to some extent for initial c. Students<br />

who had studied a Romance language produced more coronal<br />

pronunciations of initial g and c than did those who had not. Thus,<br />

when decoding word-initial letters, readers consider complicated and<br />

largely untaught patterns that extend to the ends of words.<br />

36<br />

1:50–2:05 (231)<br />

A Challenge to Chaining <strong>The</strong>ories of Serial Order: An SRN Investigation.<br />

ARIEL M. GOLDBERG, ADAM B. BUCHWALD, & BRENDA<br />

C. RAPP, Johns Hopkins University (read by Brenda C. Rapp)—Serial<br />

behavior is the production of a set of items in the proper sequence. For<br />

example, spelling is an everyday task requiring serial behavior. Prominent<br />

among theories of serial order are chaining theories, which hold<br />

that serial order is achieved through interitem associations. We used<br />

simple recurrent networks (SRNs), which are generally assumed to instantiate<br />

complex chaining, to test the chaining hypothesis of serial<br />

order in spelling. <strong>The</strong> performance of SRNs trained to spell a large<br />

corpus of words was compared with that of two dysgraphic individuals<br />

suffering from damage to the serial order component of the spelling<br />

process. Although SRNs with simulated damage produced length effects<br />

matching those of the dysgraphics, very significant differences were<br />

found for multiple features, including serial position effects. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

findings constitute an important challenge to chaining theories of serial<br />

order in spelling and, by extension, to chaining theories of serial order<br />

more generally.<br />

2:10–2:25 (232)<br />

Nature of Orthographic Representations: Disrupting the Graphemic<br />

Buffer in Unimpaired Spellers. ANGELA C. JONES & JOCELYN R.<br />

FOLK, Kent State University (read by Jocelyn R. Folk)—We investigated<br />

the hypothesis that the orthographic representations involved in<br />

spelling are multidimensional; consonant/vowel status, doubling information,<br />

and syllabic structure are part of orthographic representations<br />

(see, e.g., Caramazza & Miceli, 1990; McCloskey et al., 1994).<br />

<strong>The</strong> evidence for this hypothesis has come exclusively from studies<br />

of dysgraphic patients with damage to the graphemic buffer, the processing<br />

stage responsible for maintaining the activation of orthographic<br />

representations as serial output occurs. In the present study,<br />

we sought to examine the nature of orthographic representations in a<br />

neurologically intact sample by inducing cognitive load in unimpaired<br />

spellers, hindering the operation of the graphemic buffer. We examined<br />

the nature of the spelling errors produced under these conditions.<br />

We found convergent evidence for the multidimensional nature of orthographic<br />

representations. In addition, we found evidence of texture<br />

across and within orthographic representations; some words or letters<br />

within a word may be more strongly represented than others.<br />

2:30–2:45 (233)<br />

Even Preschool Children Show Orthographic Influences on Phonological<br />

Awareness Tasks. ANNE CASTLES, JOANNE WEDGWOOD,<br />

& VIRGINIA HOLMES, University of Melbourne—It is well established<br />

that experienced readers show influences of orthographic<br />

knowledge on their performance on tasks ostensibly tapping pure<br />

phonological awareness. For example, people tend to report that there<br />

are more phonemes in the word pitch than the word rich, because of<br />

the extra letter in the former word. Here we demonstrate that even<br />

preschool children are influenced by their emerging orthographic abilities<br />

when performing such tasks. Three- and 4-year-old children were<br />

required to identify the initial phonemes of words describing pictured<br />

objects, and their familiarity with the written form of the letters corresponding<br />

to those phonemes was assessed. An item-specific effect<br />

of letter knowledge was found: <strong>The</strong> children performed significantly<br />

better on items in the phonemic awareness test if they could identify<br />

the corresponding letter than if they could not. <strong>The</strong>se results have important<br />

implications for theories of the role of phonological awareness<br />

in reading acquisition.<br />

2:50–3:05 (234)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of Age of Acquisition on Bilingual Word Translation.<br />

J. MICHAEL BOWERS & SHELIA M. KENNISON, Oklahoma State<br />

University (read by Shelia M. Kennison)—An experiment with Spanish–<br />

English bilinguals tested the hypothesis that connections between<br />

conceptual representations and some first language (L1) words are

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!