29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Papers 214–220 Saturday Afternoon<br />

tent “cold” list, a “woman” set of prime and target lists, and a “terrorist”<br />

set of lists. <strong>The</strong>re were three opportunities to recall all target<br />

lists. Results showed no reliable difference between false reports of<br />

“cold” and those of “African American,” but both significantly exceeded<br />

false reports related to “woman” and “terrorist,” which did not<br />

differ from each other ( ps = .000 to .008).<br />

2:30–2:45 (214)<br />

False Memory for Emotionally Charged Words. PETER B. WALKER,<br />

PAUL A. HOUCK, & W. TRAMMELL NEILL, SUNY, Albany (read<br />

by W. Trammell Neill)—Participants studied lists of words that were<br />

phonologically similar to emotionally charged words (e.g., rape) or<br />

neutral words (e.g., park). <strong>The</strong> critical word was included in some<br />

studied lists, which allowed measurement of hit rates as well as false<br />

alarms on a subsequent recognition memory test. As previously found<br />

by Pesta, Murphy, and Sanders (2001), false alarms were increased for<br />

list-related charged words relative to charged words related to unstudied<br />

lists. However, hit rates for charged words were also very high,<br />

and d′ was much higher for charged words than for both neutral critical<br />

words and list words that were phonologically similar to charged<br />

words. <strong>The</strong> results suggest limitations on generalizing from the DRM<br />

list procedure to putatively false memories for emotional and traumatic<br />

events.<br />

2:50–3:05 (215)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of Strength on Source Memory Judgments of True and<br />

False Memories. IRA E. HYMAN & EDWIN ANDERSON, Western<br />

Washington University—True and false memories are predicted to differ<br />

in terms of source memory. We investigated the effect of strength<br />

on recognition and source judgments using two different methodologies:<br />

Deese/Roediger–McDermott word lists and Bransford and Franks sentences.<br />

We varied strength by presenting different numbers of items<br />

related to each topic during encoding. Strength affected recognition<br />

rates of old and new words and sentences but had little effect on source<br />

judgments. Instead, participants made source judgments based on relatedness<br />

to the topics of the studied lists. Source memory does not<br />

differentiate true and false memories and appears to be constructed.<br />

3:10–3:25 (216)<br />

Recollection Rejection of Constructed False Memories. C. J. BRAIN-<br />

ERD, V. F. REYNA, & S. ESTRADA, Cornell University—Recollection<br />

rejection is a verbatim operation of key forensic interest because it<br />

generates principled suppression of false-memory responses by neutralizing<br />

the familiarity of new events that preserve the meaning of experience.<br />

Mathematical models that partition memory processes for<br />

word lists were used to measure recollection rejection of false memories<br />

in Bransford–Franks narratives. Parameter estimates showed that<br />

recollection rejection suppressed those false memories 50% of the<br />

time, even when distractors’ surface structure and meaning were almost<br />

the same as the targets’. Manipulations that strengthen verbatim<br />

memory (e.g., target repetition, target presentation order, repeated<br />

testing) improved recollection rejection. Surprisingly high levels of<br />

recollection rejection were preserved intact over a 1-week interval in<br />

conditions that inoculated verbatim traces against disintegration. Although<br />

our findings violate constructivist theories of narrative memory,<br />

they are explained by dual-process theories that stress the opposing<br />

effects of verbatim and gist traces on false memory.<br />

3:30–3:45 (217)<br />

False Retrieval of Text. MURRAY SINGER, University of Manitoba,<br />

& GILBERT REMILLARD, Morehead State University—Our research<br />

evaluated the hypothesis that text inference retrieval receives support<br />

from a process of phantom recollection (Brainerd et al., JEP:LMC,<br />

2001) plus recollection and familiarity. We used Brainerd et al.’s conjoint<br />

recognition procedure: Three groups were respectively instructed<br />

to accept test probes if (1) they recognized them, (2) the probe<br />

matched the gist of an antecedent message, and (3) the probe was implied,<br />

but not stated, in a message. A 3-process phantom recognition<br />

34<br />

model was compared with two 2-process models: a traditional model<br />

and one that blended process dissociation and phantom recognition.<br />

For 0- versus 2-day delayed text retrieval, only the 3-process model<br />

satisfactorily fit the data, and phantom recollection was invariant with<br />

delay. In a shallow-encoding experiment, however, phantom recognition<br />

was negligible, and the 2-process models achieved satisfactory<br />

fits. <strong>The</strong>se results are consistent with a phantom-recognition process<br />

that reflects robust gist-based representations.<br />

Motor Control<br />

Dominion Ballroom, Saturday Afternoon, 1:30–3:50<br />

Chaired by Jonathan Vaughan, Hamilton College<br />

1:30–1:45 (218)<br />

Planning Trajectories in 3-D Space on the Basis of Three Postures.<br />

JONATHAN VAUGHAN, Hamilton College, MARY KLEIN BRETE-<br />

LER, Radboud University Nijmegen, DAVID A. ROSENBAUM, Pennsylvania<br />

State University, STEVEN JAX, Moss Rehabilitation Research<br />

Institute, & KIMBERLY LANTZ & ARAM KUDURSHIAN,<br />

Hamilton College—To move an arm, one must plan both a target posture<br />

and the movement trajectory to it. Forearm movements around<br />

obstacles in 2-D space have been modeled by superimposing a symmetric<br />

reversible movement to a “bounce” posture on the direct movement<br />

from start to target posture. Modeling 3-D movements is more<br />

challenging because of the noncommutativity of successive rotations<br />

about different joint axes. We now extend the obstacle-avoidance algorithm,<br />

based on three postures, to trajectories in 3-D. Eight seated<br />

subjects touched a handheld tool to a sequence of targets, sometimes<br />

circumventing an obstacle. Trajectories (with six postural degrees of<br />

freedom) were modeled using the observed start and target postures<br />

and one intermediate bounce posture. Circuitous 3-D trajectories were<br />

synthesized by superimposing a direct movement to the target (using<br />

single-axis joint rotations) and a reversible movement to a bounce<br />

posture. <strong>The</strong> results support the role of postural representations in<br />

motor planning.<br />

1:50–2:05 (219)<br />

Looking Into the Future: How We Grasp Objects Reflects Anticipation<br />

of Future Positions. DAVID A. ROSENBAUM & RAJAL G.<br />

COHEN, Pennsylvania State University—<strong>The</strong> farther into the future<br />

one looks, the less sure one can be about the usefulness of one’s plans.<br />

On the other hand, immediately forthcoming events may be anticipated<br />

with great precision. One domain where such near-term anticipation<br />

can be studied is motor control, where the way one behaves at<br />

some point in time may reflect the nature of the plan for the behavior<br />

to follow. We illustrate this approach in a series of studies on object<br />

manipulation. Our subjects reached for an object, knowing they<br />

would transport it to other locations. How they took hold of the object<br />

reflected considerable sensitivity to future states. <strong>The</strong> results indicate<br />

that behavioral methods are important for studying motor planning<br />

and that the study of motor planning may reveal much about<br />

cognition.<br />

2:10–2:25 (220)<br />

Fitts’s Law in Actual and Imagined Action: A Closer Look.<br />

SUZANNE M. GRILLI & ANDREW B. SLIFKIN, Cleveland State<br />

University (read by Andrew B. Slifkin)—A large literature suggests<br />

substantial overlap of the processes involved in actual and imagined<br />

action. For example, in a Fitts aiming paradigm, actual and imagined<br />

movement time (MT) was essentially identical at common levels of<br />

the index of difficulty (ID) (see, e.g., Sirigu et al., 1996). Here, we<br />

extend that research by examining performance over a broadened<br />

range of target amplitudes (A) and widths (W): ID ranged from 1.00<br />

to 5.00 bits, and W ranged from 0.13 to 2.16 cm within each ID level.<br />

MT always increased with ID, and within each ID level MT was constant<br />

across the larger values of W. Those results confirm Fitts’s law.<br />

However, MT tended to lengthen with further reductions in W. This

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!