29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Papers 8–14 Friday Morning<br />

of encoding conditions, we examined, in three experiments, whether<br />

part-list cuing is a transient or lasting phenomenon. Across the experiments,<br />

the detrimental effect of part-list cues was consistently<br />

found to be transient with associative encoding and lasting with<br />

nonassociative encoding. <strong>The</strong>se results indicate that the persistence of<br />

part-list cuing depends on encoding, thus challenging both strategy<br />

disruption and retrieval inhibition as general accounts of part-list<br />

cuing. A two-mechanism account is provided, according to which the<br />

two mechanisms mediate the effect in different encoding conditions.<br />

8:40–8:55 (8)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Influence of Preexisting Associations on Integration Effects<br />

in Directed Forgetting. LILI SAHAKYAN, University of North Carolina,<br />

Greensboro, & LEILANI B. GOODMON, University of South<br />

Florida—<strong>The</strong> directed-forgetting effect refers to impaired recall of<br />

previously studied information as a result of instructing people to forget.<br />

Previous research showed that integration resulting from strong primary<br />

associations between to-be-forgotten (TBF) and to-be-remembered<br />

(TBR) lists reduces the directed-forgetting effect (Conway, Harries,<br />

Noyes, Racsmany, & Frankish, 2000). <strong>The</strong> present studies investigated<br />

the effects of the direction and strength of preexisting semantic associations<br />

(as measured by free association norms) on directed forgetting.<br />

In a series of studies, the TBF and TBR lists were completely unrelated<br />

(rose–business), linked only in one direction (stem activates<br />

rose, but rose does not activate stem), or linked in both directions<br />

(rose–petals). Results revealed no forgetting in the bidirectional condition<br />

only. To determine whether greater net strength of the associations<br />

or the presence of links in both directions were responsible for<br />

these findings, subsequent studies crossed strength with the direction<br />

of association.<br />

9:00–9:15 (9)<br />

Protecting Against Inhibition: <strong>The</strong> Influence of Episodic and Semantic<br />

Integration on Retrieval-Induced Forgetting. LEILANI B.<br />

GOODMON, University of South Florida, & MICHAEL C. ANDER-<br />

SON, University of Oregon (read by Michael C. Anderson)—When<br />

people form episodic connections between memories that share a cue, the<br />

tendency for those memories to compete is reduced. This episodic integration<br />

has been shown to protect memories from retrieval-induced<br />

forgetting (RIF), a phenomenon in which retrieval of some associates<br />

of a cue suppresses other competitors. Here, we report three experiments<br />

examining whether semantic integration (i.e., preexisting associations),<br />

also protects against RIF, even when no episodic integration<br />

is attempted. In the interset condition, strong a priori associations<br />

existed between the retrieval practiced and nonpracticed sets. In the<br />

intraset condition, there were strong associations within the practiced<br />

set and nonpracticed set. Results show a striking dependency of inhibition<br />

on the pattern of associations: <strong>The</strong> intraset condition revealed<br />

substantial inhibition, but the interset condition revealed none. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

findings show that integration—whether episodic or semantic—forms<br />

a fundamental boundary condition on RIF that is likely to explain variability<br />

in this phenomenon.<br />

9:20–9:35 (10)<br />

In Search of Inhibition in Retroactive “Inhibition.” DENNIS J.<br />

DELPRATO & KAREN STANLEY, Eastern Michigan University—<br />

Although retroactive interference (RI) once was routinely referred to<br />

as retroactive inhibition, convincing evidence of inhibition in the classic<br />

RI preparation may have never been reported. This research, in<br />

which experimental subjects studied two successive lists of 20 different<br />

unrelated words, was designed to rule out several noninhibitory<br />

sources of RI decrements. And one of the two RI experimental conditions<br />

(study only), like the RI control condition, should not have activated<br />

inhibitory processes, because subjects did not overtly recall<br />

second-list words prior to the test of first-list retention. <strong>The</strong> other<br />

(study–test) was expected to activate inhibitory control processes, due<br />

to overt retrieval of words during second-list study. Over two experi-<br />

2<br />

ments, RI was evident, but only when the second list was studied for<br />

more than a single cycle, provided that the words were also recalled<br />

in conjunction with study episodes. <strong>The</strong>se findings seem to suggest<br />

that inhibition does participate in RI.<br />

9:40–9:55 (11)<br />

Repression Revisited. MICHAEL S. HUMPHREYS & ANGELA M.<br />

MAGUIRE, University of Queensland, & DOUGLAS L. NELSON,<br />

University of South Florida—It has been suggested that the think/nothink<br />

paradigm in which participants are encouraged to suppress responses<br />

(Anderson & Green, 2001) may provide a model for the<br />

Freudian concept of repression. We have been able to replicate the<br />

basic results through mere exposure, without having subjects practice<br />

suppressing a response. We argue that our results, as well as the Anderson<br />

and Green results, are better explained by associative chaining,<br />

where interference occurs when a nontarget member of the chain<br />

is associated with context.<br />

Causal Learning and Reasoning<br />

Grand Ballroom West, Friday Morning, 8:00–10:00<br />

Chaired by Brett K. Hayes, University of New South Wales<br />

8:00–8:15 (12)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Development of Causal Induction. BRETT K. HAYES & SUSAN<br />

P. THOMPSON, University of New South Wales—Most process models<br />

of category-based induction assume that the strength of an inductive<br />

argument is a positive function of the similarity between the features<br />

of premise and conclusion instances. This paper examines the<br />

development of an alternate basis for induction: shared causal relations<br />

in premises and conclusions. Three experiments compared the<br />

use of feature similarity and causal relations in the inductive reasoning<br />

of 5- and 8-year-olds and adults, using artificial category stimuli.<br />

Although the prevalence of induction based on causal relations increased<br />

with age, 5-year-olds showed a reliable preference for causal<br />

over similarity-based induction. For all ages, inductive inferences<br />

based on causal antecedent features were stronger than those based on<br />

causal consequent features. Similar results were found across two conceptual<br />

domains (animals and artifacts). <strong>The</strong> implications for models<br />

of inductive reasoning and inductive development are considered.<br />

8:20–8:35 (13)<br />

Mechanisms of Induction Across Points of Development. VLADIMIR<br />

M. SLOUTSKY, Ohio State University—<strong>The</strong> ability to generalize from<br />

known to novel appears early in development. To examine mechanisms<br />

of inductive generalization, Sloutsky and Fisher (2004) studied<br />

recognition memory for items, using the induction-then-recognition<br />

(ITR) paradigm. Adults induced on the basis of category information,<br />

whereas 5-year-olds induced on the basis of similarity. <strong>The</strong> reported<br />

research further examines the mechanisms underlying induction across<br />

points of development. We presented young children and adults with<br />

the ITR task, while recording their eye movements. Overall results indicate<br />

that (1) all participants accurately performed the induction task,<br />

(2) children had multiple fixations on the studied items and accurately<br />

discriminated studied items and new members of studied categories,<br />

and (3) adults had few fixations on the studied items and poorly discriminated<br />

studied items and new members of studied categories.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings indicate that children’s induction is based on processing<br />

of item-specific information, whereas adults’ induction is based<br />

on processing of category information.<br />

8:40–8:55 (14)<br />

Children’s Reasoning Under Uncertainty: <strong>The</strong> Influence of Conditional<br />

and Unconditional Contingencies. EMMA L. JOHNSTONE,<br />

MICHELLE R. ELLEFSON, & NICK CHATER, University of Warwick<br />

(read by Michelle R. Ellefson)—In previous studies, causal contingencies<br />

have been suggested to play an important role in causality judg-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!