29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Friday Afternoon Papers 105–111<br />

time within a trial before another tone was generated was 1 sec. Two<br />

additional subjects replicated the early stages of learning. <strong>The</strong> data<br />

provide a baseline for the learning and relearning of musical tone<br />

identification/discrimination. It appears that despite the aging process<br />

(the subject got older), the ability to reach maximum levels of accuracy<br />

did not decrease.<br />

Visual Short-Term Memory<br />

Grand Ballroom Centre, Friday Afternoon, 3:50–5:30<br />

Chaired by Pierre Jolicœur, Université de Montréal<br />

3:50–4:05 (105)<br />

Tracking the Involvement of Visual Short-Term Memory Using<br />

Human Electrophysiology. PIERRE JOLICŒUR, BENOIT BRISSON,<br />

NICOLAS ROBITAILLE, ÉMILIE LEBLANC, ROSALIE PERRON,<br />

& DAVID PRIME, Université de Montréal—Elegant work by Vogel<br />

and Machizawa (2004) suggests that maintaining information selected<br />

for storage in visual short-term memory (VSTM) causes a sustained<br />

posterior contralateral negativity (SPCN) in event-related potentials,<br />

reflecting neural activity associated with memory retention. We observed<br />

similar SPCNs in ERPs from several experiments designed to<br />

measure the N2pc (another interesting ERP strongly correlated with<br />

visual spatial selective attention). In this paper, we review several spatial<br />

selective attention experiments in which we observed both N2pc<br />

and SPCN ERPs. We also present new experiments designed to dissociate<br />

the N2pc from the SPCN and to use the SPCN as an ongoing<br />

measure of VSTM involvement in visual cognition.<br />

4:10–4:25 (106)<br />

Dissociable Neural Mechanisms Supporting Visual Short-Term<br />

Memory for Objects. YAODA XU & MARVIN M. CHUN, Yale University—<strong>The</strong>re<br />

is growing debate contesting whether visual shortterm<br />

memory (VSTM) capacity is variable or limited to a fixed number<br />

of objects. Using fMRI, we show that while activations in the<br />

superior intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the lateral occipital complex<br />

(LOC) track the number of objects held in VSTM as object feature<br />

complexity vary, those in the inferior IPS increase with set size and<br />

plateau at set size 4 regardless of object feature complexity. Moreover,<br />

while inferior IPS representations are spatial in nature, those of the<br />

superior IPS and the LOC are not. <strong>The</strong>se results suggest a two-stage<br />

model of VSTM whereby a fixed number of objects are first selected<br />

via their locations by the inferior IPS, and then a variable number of<br />

objects, depending on visual complexity, are subsequently retained in<br />

VSTM by the superior IPS and the LOC. VSTM capacity is, therefore,<br />

determined both by a fixed number of objects and by object complexity.<br />

4:30–4:45 (107)<br />

Working Memory Capacity and Control of Visual Search. BRAD-<br />

LEY J. POOLE & MICHAEL J. KANE, University of North Carolina,<br />

Greensboro (read by Michael J. Kane)—<strong>The</strong> executive attention theory<br />

of working memory (WM) capacity proposes that WM span tasks<br />

predict individual differences in fluid cognitive abilities because span<br />

reflects a domain-general attention control ability (Engle & Kane,<br />

2004). Supportive evidence comes from findings that individual differences<br />

in WM capacity predict performance on tasks requiring conscious<br />

control of habitual responses and visual attention. However, we<br />

have previously reported that WM capacity is not related to prototypical<br />

visual search performance, even in tasks eliciting substantial RT<br />

slopes. Because attention deployment in typical search tasks is not volitional<br />

(Wolfe et al., 2000), here, we designed two visual search tasks<br />

to increase demands on executive control—one requiring the volitional<br />

movement of attention around a search display and one requiring<br />

the restriction of search to discontiguous locations amid distractors.<br />

Results indicate that WM capacity predicts the ability to<br />

discontiguously focus visual attention, especially when focus must be<br />

reconfigured across successive trials.<br />

17<br />

4:50–5:05 (108)<br />

Errors in Visuospatial Working Memory. CESARE CORNOLDI &<br />

ROSSANA DE BENI, University of Padua, & NICOLA MAM-<br />

MARELLA, University of Chieti—It is common knowledge that elderly<br />

people have difficulty in performing working memory tasks, including<br />

visuospatial working memory (VSWM) ones. Unfortunately,<br />

the generality and the characteristics of this difficulty have not yet<br />

been deeply examined. A series of experiments investigated the active<br />

component of VSWM in young and elderly people. Participants were<br />

shown sequences of matrices on which three positions were pointed<br />

at; their task was to process all the positions and then to indicate only<br />

the final positions of each sequence. Results showed that the success<br />

in active VSWM tasks depends—at least partially—on difficulties in<br />

discriminating between items having a different status and, in particular,<br />

in avoiding intrusions (i.e., avoiding the recall of information already<br />

activated).<br />

5:10–5:25 (109)<br />

Similarity and Interference in Visual Working Memory. STEVEN J.<br />

LUCK, PO-HAN LIN, & ANDREW HOLLINGWORTH, University<br />

of Iowa—Similarity leads to interference between representations in<br />

long-term memory and in verbal working memory. Does it also lead<br />

to interference between representations in visual working memory?<br />

To answer this question, we conducted a color change detection experiment<br />

in which the items in a given sample display were either similar<br />

to or different from each other in hue. Accuracy was not impaired<br />

when the colors were similar to each other; instead, accuracy was improved.<br />

Superior accuracy was also observed for similar items when<br />

the to-be-remembered items were presented sequentially, even when<br />

the first item in the sequence was tested. <strong>The</strong> increased accuracy for<br />

similar items does not, therefore, reflect a more accurate initial encoding<br />

but, instead, reflects differences in the maintenance or comparison<br />

processes. Thus, similarity between concurrent representations<br />

in visual working memory influences performance, but the effect<br />

is the opposite of the interference effects observed with other forms<br />

of memory.<br />

Prospective Memory<br />

Grand Ballroom East, Friday Afternoon, 4:10–5:30<br />

Chaired by Bob Uttl, University of Tamagawa<br />

4:10–4:25 (110)<br />

Quo Vadis, Prospective Memory? BOB UTTL, University of Tamagawa—Prospective<br />

memory proper (ProMP) brings back to awareness<br />

previously formed plans at the right place and time; ProMP is distinct<br />

from other subdomains of prospective memory (ProM), such as vigilance/<br />

monitoring. A quantitative review of over 200 age contrasts from published<br />

studies revealed that although both ProMP and vigilance show<br />

substantial age declines, the declines in ProMP are larger than those<br />

in vigilance/monitoring and in retrospective memory. Strikingly, the<br />

size of observed age declines on ProM tasks is directly related to the<br />

degree to which researchers are able to avoid ceiling effects (r 2 > .40,<br />

p < .0001). Furthermore, studies that bias task demands to benefit<br />

older versus younger adults show smaller age effects. In sum, age declines<br />

have been underestimated, due to methodological shortcomings,<br />

including ceiling-limited scores, age confounds, and inappropriate<br />

effect size measures. Finally, a failure to distinguish the<br />

multiple subdomains of ProM contributes to literary confusion and<br />

theoretical quagmire.<br />

4:30–4:45 (111)<br />

Age Invariance in Prospective Memory: Focal Cues and Resource<br />

Allocation. MARK A. MCDANIEL, Washington University, & GILLES<br />

O. EINSTEIN, Furman University—In two experiments, we varied how<br />

focal the prospective memory cue was to the ongoing task. We found<br />

no age differences in prospective memory. Importantly, for focal cues,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!