29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Papers 98–104 Friday Afternoon<br />

prime with the same main verb, as compared with when it is preceded<br />

by a DO prime with the same verb. However, Experiment 2 shows that<br />

garden pathing is not reduced when presentation lists include distractor<br />

sentence pairs that also repeat verbs but vary in syntactic structure<br />

or verb sense from first to second sentence. Thus, while Experiment 1<br />

extends structural priming in comprehension to the reduction of the<br />

DO/SC ambiguity, results of Experiment 2 suggest that structural<br />

priming in comprehension is mediated, in part, by learning that verb<br />

repetition may signal structural repetition.<br />

3:30–3:45 (98)<br />

Motor Resonance in the Comprehension of Action Sentences.<br />

ROLF A. ZWAAN & LARRY J. TAYLOR, Florida State University—<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is evidence that observing actions and understanding sentences<br />

about actions activate corresponding motor processes in the observer/<br />

comprehender. In five experiments, we examine this phenomenon of<br />

motor resonance further. Specifically, we address two novel questions.<br />

<strong>The</strong> first question is whether visual motion associated with an action<br />

produces motor resonance in sentence comprehension. <strong>The</strong> second<br />

question concerns the modulation of motor resonance during sentence<br />

comprehension. Experiment 1 shows that a rotating visual stimulus affects<br />

manual rotation. Experiment 2 shows that comprehending a sentence<br />

about manual rotation (e.g., “He turned down the volume”) affects<br />

sensibility judgments made by actual manual rotation. Experiment 3<br />

shows that a rotating visual stimulus affects the comprehension of<br />

manual rotation sentences. Experiments 4 and 5 show that motor resonance<br />

is a rather immediate and localized phenomenon, exclusively<br />

occurring during the main verb of the sentence that specifies the rotation<br />

direction.<br />

3:50–4:05 (99)<br />

ERP and Joint Time–Frequency Responses to Grammatical and<br />

Semantic Violations in Dutch–English Bilinguals. DOUGLAS J.<br />

DAVIDSON & PETER INDEFREY, F. C. Donders Centre for Cognitive<br />

Neuroimaging—Previous research on multilingual sentence processing<br />

has shown that event-related potential (ERP) responses to<br />

grammatical or semantic violations in sentences presented in a second<br />

language are delayed or more variable than first-language responses.<br />

We examined responses to grammatical and semantic violations<br />

in Dutch speakers who were proficient in English, to investigate<br />

whether joint time–frequency analyses can provide addition insight<br />

into the nature of second-language sentence processing. <strong>The</strong> analyses<br />

indicated that grammatical violations led to a power reduction with a<br />

relatively broad distribution at approximately 10–15 Hz following the<br />

violation, relative to the controls, and a corresponding increase in<br />

power at approximately 3–5 Hz. Analyses of the ERP results indicated<br />

an N400 effect for semantic violation critical words, relative to controls,<br />

as well as a P600 effect for the grammatical violations. <strong>The</strong><br />

event-related changes in power suggest that grammatical violations<br />

incur a greater attentional load in bilinguals.<br />

Counting, Timing, and Time Perception<br />

Conference Rooms B&C, Friday Afternoon, 1:30–3:10<br />

Chaired by Richard A. Carlson, Pennsylvania State University<br />

1:30–1:45 (100)<br />

Counting on Emotion. RICHARD A. CARLSON & RACHEL E.<br />

SMITH, Pennsylvania State University—Emotion is thought to play<br />

roles in the control of cognitive processes at scales ranging from the<br />

guidance or biasing of attention to the selection of decision-making<br />

strategies. We considered hypotheses about the role of emotion in control<br />

and error monitoring of a routine cognitive task, event counting.<br />

Participants in several experiments counted sequences of briefly presented<br />

schematic faces that could be happy, neutral, or sad. Counting<br />

was generally more accurate with faces than with other stimuli. More<br />

interesting, the emotionality of the faces being counted influenced the<br />

patterns of error types (undercounts vs. overcounts) and the success<br />

16<br />

with which participants monitored their performance for errors. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

effects depended on other factors known to influence event counting<br />

and are unlikely to be due to shifts in general mood. <strong>The</strong> results point<br />

to a role for emotionality in the online intentional control of fluent<br />

performance.<br />

1:50–2:05 (101)<br />

Adults’ Age-Related Differences in Approximate Quantification.<br />

DELPHINE GANDINI & PATRICK LEMAIRE, CNRS and Université<br />

de Provence (read by Patrick Lemaire)—Young and older adults had<br />

to find the approximate number of dots for collections of between 11<br />

to 98 dots within 6 sec. On the basis of previous works, participants<br />

could choose between two strategies in a choice condition: visual<br />

scanning strategy (i.e., participants scanned the collection of dots and<br />

gave an approximate numerosity without counting dots) or anchoring<br />

strategy (i.e., participants first enumerated a few dots and then visually<br />

estimated the remaining dots). Participants were also forced to use<br />

each strategy on all trials in no-choice conditions. Data showed agerelated<br />

changes in strategy use (percent use of each strategy) and strategy<br />

adaptivity (i.e., choosing the best strategy on each item), in strategy<br />

performance (latency and accuracy) and in eye-movement data<br />

(number and duration of fixations). Implications of these findings for<br />

the further understanding of approximate quantification skills and the<br />

effects of age on these skills will be discussed.<br />

2:10–2:25 (102)<br />

Interval Length and Event-Counting Errors. DANIEL N. CASSENTI<br />

& RICHARD A. CARLSON, Pennsylvania State University—<strong>The</strong><br />

speed–accuracy tradeoff specifies that as speed of processing increases,<br />

the chance of error increases. Experiment 1 demonstrated that as presentation<br />

speed of to-be-counted events decreases, the chance of a<br />

counting error increases, suggesting a revision of the speed–accuracy<br />

tradeoff. Experiment 2 distinguished between two causes of this effect:<br />

forgetting or overuse of a strategy of rehearsal to prevent forgetting.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results implicate the overuse of rehearsal and suggest that<br />

the speed–accuracy tradeoff is a special case of a more general<br />

speed–accuracy relation. Results are discussed in relation to the nature<br />

of goals, cognitive control, and error detection.<br />

2:30–2:45 (103)<br />

Auditory Kappa: Effects of Pitch on Perceived Time. NOAH<br />

MACKENZIE & MARI R. JONES, Ohio State University (read by<br />

Mari R. Jones)—<strong>The</strong> kappa effect refers to a phenomenon whereby<br />

spatial relationships among stimuli affect judgments of temporal relationships<br />

among those stimuli. Events widely separated in space will<br />

be perceived as being further apart in time than events spaced closer<br />

together. One explanation for this appeals to the apparent motionlike<br />

properties of the stimuli. Three experiments on auditory kappa were<br />

conducted. In Experiment 1, a listener judged whether the second tone<br />

in a sequence of three (a kappa cell) was closer in time to the first or<br />

the third tone. Listeners were most accurate when the pitch of the second<br />

tone was closest to the first or third tone. Experiments 2 and 3<br />

added serial context immediately preceding the kappa cell. It was<br />

found that a serial context preserving the motionlike properties of the<br />

kappa cell preserved the kappa effect, whereas a context that did not<br />

preserve these properties abolished it.<br />

2:50–3:05 (104)<br />

A 20-Year Longitudinal Study of Musical Tone Discrimination and<br />

Identification. SHELDON G. LEVY, Wayne State University—Eartraining<br />

experiments have been reported for more than a century. However,<br />

past research has investigated the phenomenon over relatively<br />

short periods of time. This report is based on 20 years of identification/<br />

discrimination of randomly generated tones. Intervals without practice<br />

ranged from 1 day to 5 years. Tones were generated within a twooctave<br />

range (approximately normal distribution around G above middle<br />

C) with 60–80 tones per minute, depending upon the speed of<br />

response and the accuracy of identification. <strong>The</strong> maximum allowable

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!