29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 5059–5065 Saturday Evening<br />

processes requires substantial resources. Our present research assesses<br />

both methodological and theoretical issues related to the relative<br />

immunity of retrieval processes to the detrimental effects of divided<br />

attention. With respect to methodological issues, we evaluated<br />

the possibility that retrieval processes show this resilience because the<br />

concurrent tasks used are often not very demanding. <strong>The</strong>oretically, we<br />

evaluated the degree to which retrieval, despite not being much affected<br />

by the difficulty of the concurrent task, is vulnerable to competition<br />

when the type of processing required by the concurrent task<br />

is the same as in the retrieval task.<br />

(5059)<br />

Enhancing Recollection Without Enhancing Recognition Probability:<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effects of Testing on Recollection and Familiarity. JASON<br />

C. K. CHAN & KATHLEEN B. MCDERMOTT, Washington University<br />

(sponsored by Janet Duchek)—We examined the effects of taking an<br />

initial test on later memory retrieval, using a modified process dissociation<br />

procedure. Subjects studied unrelated word lists and performed<br />

immediate free recall on half the lists; for the remaining lists,<br />

subjects performed an unrelated filler task. Subjects then performed<br />

delayed recognition tests in which the relative contribution of recollection<br />

and familiarity can be estimated. Similar to previous findings,<br />

overall recognition performance (hits and correct rejections) showed<br />

no testing effect. However, when recollection and familiarity estimates<br />

were computed via source judgments, taking an initial free recall<br />

test was shown to enhance recollection (or source memory) without<br />

affecting familiarity. A separate analysis comparing recalled and<br />

unrecalled items showed that this enhanced recollection was seen only<br />

for words recalled initially, suggesting that the origin of the testing effect<br />

may lie in subjects’ remembering the previous recall episode,<br />

rather than the study episode.<br />

(5060)<br />

Sensory Modality Moderates Illusions of Recognition Based on Perceptual<br />

Fluency but Not Conceptual Fluency. DEANNE L. WEST-<br />

ERMAN, SUNY, Binghamton, JEREMY K. MILLER, Willamette University,<br />

& MARIANNE E. LLOYD, SUNY, Binghamton—Illusions of<br />

recognition memory based on enhanced perceptual fluency are sensitive<br />

to the perceptual match between the study and the test phases of<br />

an experiment. Enhanced visual fluency of test stimuli leads to an illusion<br />

of recognition only when the study list is also visual. Five experiments<br />

demonstrate that participants are very reluctant to interpret<br />

enhanced perceptual fluency as a sign of recognition when there has<br />

been a change in sensory modality across the study and test phases of<br />

the experiment. <strong>The</strong> present results show that the interaction between<br />

modality and perceptual fluency manipulations persisted under<br />

speeded recognition conditions, with a long delay between study and<br />

test, and also when participants visualized each study word as it was<br />

presented on an auditory list. Results also show that illusions of recognition<br />

based on a more conceptually driven manipulation of processing<br />

fluency are not sensitive to modality changes between study and<br />

test.<br />

(5061)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Contribution of Familiarity and Recollection to Immediate<br />

and Continuous-Distractor Recognition. YONATAN GOSHEN-<br />

GOTTSTEIN & AMIT G. STEINBERG, Tel-Aviv University—Using<br />

single-probe recognition, an earlier study (Goshen-Gottstein & Talmi,<br />

<strong>2005</strong>) reported a recency effect not only when study items were presented<br />

sequentially and testing was immediate (i.e., immediate recognition),<br />

but also when distractor activity was interspersed between<br />

study items and also followed the last study item (i.e., continuousdistractor<br />

recognition). In this study, we examined the contributions<br />

of recollection and familiarity to performance in these two recognition<br />

tasks. Following each study list, participants’ memory was<br />

probed for a single position. For every participant, each list position<br />

was probed an equal number of times across lists. For positive responses,<br />

participants made know/remember/guess judgments. Results<br />

132<br />

replicated the recency effect in both tasks. Critically, the contribution<br />

of familiarity was similar in both tasks, with differences in performance<br />

being mediated only by recollection.<br />

• IMPLICIT MEMORY •<br />

(5062)<br />

An Interaction Between Priming and Location of Target Information<br />

in Reading. SUSAN T. DAVIS, CHRISTINA G. RUTHERFORD, &<br />

DEBORAH L. BAKOWSKI, University of Dayton—In accord with<br />

extant research (e.g., Rastle & Coltheart, 1999), phonological priming<br />

produced greater accuracy and shorter reaction times overall than<br />

semantic priming in a task that required scanning text for information.<br />

However, this advantage was weakest for information embedded at the<br />

end of a text passage. Perceptual selectivity (cf. Masson, 1982) provides<br />

a reasonable explanation for the advantage of phonological<br />

priming when information that can answer a specific question is<br />

scanned for. However, we propose that the reader’s initial goal may be<br />

affected by the text itself. Longer texts appear to engage the reader in<br />

the textual story line and produce a switch to conceptually selective<br />

processing. Consequently, as reading of the text progresses, semantic<br />

priming becomes more advantageous for speeded identification of information<br />

found in the latter portion of the text. This is a reasonable<br />

strategy that appears to operate as much on an implicit level as the<br />

priming itself.<br />

(5063)<br />

Evoking Familiarity Without Recollection: Modeling the Déjà Vu<br />

Experience. ALAN S. BROWN, Southern Methodist University, &<br />

ELIZABETH J. MARSH, Duke University—We attempted to model<br />

a feeling of false positive familiarity similar to the déjà vu experience.<br />

After shallow exposure to pictures of naturalistic scenes (searching for<br />

an embedded cross), participants returned 1 (Experiment 1) or 3 (Experiment<br />

2) weeks later to evaluate whether they felt that they had actually<br />

visited each pictured setting (new plus old scenes). Mundane<br />

and unique campus scenes from an unfamiliar campus (Duke or SMU)<br />

were shown zero, one, or two times in the first session. <strong>The</strong> mean visit<br />

rating increased with prior exposure, but one versus two exposures did<br />

not differ. Participants gave higher visit ratings to mundane than to<br />

unique scenes, and this did not vary as a function of exposure frequency<br />

or delay. About half of each sample reported “yes” to the question<br />

“did you have a déjà vu response to any of the photos?”<br />

(5064)<br />

Implicit Preference in the Absence of Conscious Memory for Advertisements<br />

Appearing in Basketball Games. KRISTEN A.<br />

DILIBERTO-MACALUSO, Berry College, TODD A. KAHAN, Bates<br />

College, & ROSANNA PRATO, Berry College—We examined unconscious<br />

(implicit) perception and memory of advertisements that<br />

appeared in the background of basketball games. Participants were instructed<br />

to attend either to the ads (deliberate group) or to a salient<br />

aspect of the video clip itself (incidental group). Once all 18 clips had<br />

been shown, we presented participants with 36 advertisements, which<br />

they rated on four dimensions (eye-catching, appealing, memorable,<br />

distinctive), using a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = very much less than<br />

average; 7 = very much more than average). In addition, participants<br />

indicated whether or not they remembered each ad from the original<br />

video clip. We found that participants in the incidental group rated<br />

“old” logos as more appealing and distinctive than “new” logos; however,<br />

recognition memory did not differ for old and new logos. <strong>The</strong><br />

data suggest that participants unconsciously showed a preference for<br />

the original advertisements but showed inattentional blindness for<br />

recognition memory of those same ads.<br />

(5065)<br />

Differential Effects of Time of Day on Explicit and Implicit Memory.<br />

GILLIAN ROWE & STEVEN VALDERRAMA, University of Toronto,<br />

LYNN HASHER, University of Toronto and Rotman Research Institute,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!