Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Posters 5001–5008 Saturday Evening POSTER SESSION V Sheraton Hall, Saturday Evening, 6:00–7:30 • VISUAL PROCESSING • (5001) The Poggendorff Illusion: Rectangle Fill Patterns Alter Illusion Magnitude. CLARE K. PORAC, Pennsylvania State University, Erie, & ALAN SEARLEMAN, St. Lawrence University—Masini et al. (1992) found that illusion magnitude (IM) in the Poggendorff is lowest when the inducing rectangle is filled with lines resembling the figure’s oblique line. IM is highest when the rectangle is filled with lines that mimic the straight lines in the inducing rectangle. In Experiment 1, six Poggendorff figures were presented with vertical rectangles that were filled with vertical lines of high and low spatial frequency or horizontal lines of high and low spatial frequency or were solid white or black. In Experiment 2, the same six Poggendorff figures were presented with a horizontal rectangle. IM was highest in Experiment 1 when the rectangle was filled with low spatial frequency vertical lines. In Experiment 2, IM was highest when the rectangle was filled with vertical lines. Vertical line fill causes higher IM regardless of the orientation of the straight lines in the rectangle. (5002) ART Theory Accounts for Perceived Changes in Perspective Pictures Due to Eye Height. IGOR JURICEVIC & JOHN M. KENNEDY, University of Toronto, Scarborough—A problem that has been with the study of vision since the Renaissance is our reaction to perspective in pictures. Perspective pictures of square tiles can be drawn using a high, low, or intermediate eye height. When the eye height is low—that is, close to the ground—many quadrilaterals that depict squares appear compressed. But increase the eye height and the apparent compression decreases, producing quadrilaterals that appear square or even elongated. A two-factor angles and ratios together (ART) theory explains which quadrilaterals appear compressed, square, or elongated across the different eye heights. The ART theory’s two factors are (1) the ratio of the visual angles of the tile’s sides and (2) the angle between the normal to the picture plane from the observer and the direction to the tile from the observer. We point out implications for constancy. (5003) Asynchronous Priming in the Gamma Band Facilitates Discrimination of Kanizsa-Type Forms. ROBERT G. MORRISON, Xunesis, & HONGJING LU, JOHN E. HUMMEL, & KEITH J. HOLYOAK, UCLA—We investigated the role of temporally structured priming in discrimination tasks involving perceptual relations between multiple Kanizsa-type figures. Visual information presented as asynchronous flicker in the gamma band can modulate the perception of multiple objects in a subsequent display, decreasing response time to identify and discriminate relations between the objects. Our findings provide support for the JIM model of visual binding. (5004) The Flash-Lag Effect Is Modulated by Accumulating Preprobe Excitation and Postprobe Inhibition. MICAH GEER & WILLIAM C. SCHMIDT, SUNY, Buffalo—The flash-lag effect occurs when observers perceive a flashed probe to lag spatially behind a moving target even though both were physically aligned at the time of the flash. The present research manipulated properties of the target theorized to modulate the speed at which signals from moving stimuli are neurally processed. The results are at odds with a number of explanations of the effect and are predicted exclusively by an account that relies on accumulating subthreshold excitation and cumulative lateral inhibition induced in cortical maps by the moving stimulus. (5005) Eye-Movement–Based Biometric Measures. FRANK M. MARCHAK & TANNER L. KEIL, Veridical Research and Design—Kasprowski and 124 Ober (2005) describe a method for characterizing eye movements to a moving point that can capture differences among individuals and uniquely identify a specific individual. In this work, we attempt to produce a similar result by characterizing eye movements to static images of natural scenes. Standard eye movement measures based on fixation and saccade data were examined for subjects repeatedly viewing the same images. A selection of classification techniques were applied to these data to determine a set of parameters that could uniquely identify an individual. We present the results of these efforts, both positive and negative, and discuss the implications of the use of eye movements as a biometric measure. (5006) The Development of Sensitivity to a Common Region as a Perceptual Organizational Principle. ANGELA HAYDEN & RAMESH S. BHATT, University of Kentucky, & PAUL C. QUINN, University of Delaware—We examined whether 3- to 4-month-olds and 6- to 7month-olds exhibit sensitivity to a common region as an organizing principle. Infants were familiarized to two rectangles, one of which contained two stars. Older infants looked longer at a novel pattern in which one of the stars was now located in the other rectangle than at a pattern in which the star was displaced the same amount but still within the same rectangle. Infants in a control condition in which the organization was not based on a common region failed to discriminate between analogously modified patterns. Thus, 6- to 7-month-olds exhibited evidence of sensitivity to a common region. However, 3- to 4month-olds failed to exhibit similar sensitivity in this study and in a subsequent study with simpler patterns. These results indicate that the principle of common region is functional in infancy, although not until sometime after 3–4 months of age. (5007) Configural Processing of Faces and Objects: An ERP Study of the Thatcher Illusion. LUC BOUTSEN, Aston University, & GLYN W. HUMPHREYS, PETER PRAAMSTRA, & TRACY WARBRICK, University of Birmingham—In the Thatcher illusion, a face with inverted eyes and mouth looks abnormal when upright, but not when inverted. Behavioral studies have shown that Thatcherization of an upright face disrupts perceptual processing of the local configuration. We studied ERP correlates of the illusion during the perception of faces and nonface objects, to investigate whether inversion and Thatcherization affect similar neural mechanisms. Observers passively viewed faces and houses in four conditions (upright vs. inverted and normal vs. Thatcherized) while detecting an oddball category (chairs). Thatcherization modulated N170 to faces, but not to houses, over the occipito-temporal cortex. This modulation matched the illusion, since it was larger for upright than for inverted faces; it occurred for whole faces and parts. P1 was unaffected by Thatcherization but was modulated by inversion. Face Thatcherization affected P2, whereas inversion affected P2 across categories. These results suggest that inversion and Thatcherization disrupt perceptual encoding of faces and objects differently. (5008) Do Affordances Change With Changes in Body Shape? WILLIAM LANGSTON & CHARLOTTE TIDRICK, Middle Tennessee State University—Patients undergoing weight loss surgery frequently report a mismatch between their new body shape and their perceived shape (affecting such decisions as which chairs can hold them or whether they can walk down the aisle in an airplane). Preliminary research was conducted to determine what might influence changes in affordances after changes in body shape. For an initial study using a method of limits procedure, participants were asked to judge whether or not they could fit through an aperture when facing forward or sideways or carrying a large backpack. Participants were then asked to pass through the aperture to determine the smallest gap through which they could actually fit. Participants were slightly more accurate for facing forward than for facing sideways or carrying a backpack. Additional studies will investigate improvements to the methodology and how af-

Saturday Evening Posters 5009–5015 fordances change with changes in body shape (e.g., becoming more accurate while a well-stuffed backpack is worn). (5009) Changes in Visual Awareness: Event-Related Brain Potential Correlates. HARTMUT LEUTHOLD, University of Glasgow—In a recent event-related brain potential (ERP) study (Koivistu & Revonsuo, 2003), awareness of visual change has been associated with an early negative ERP deflection over posterior electrodes. This study employed rectangular stimuli that varied only in orientation. Thus, to investigate whether the change-related ERP negativity is independent of the object dimension changing, I conducted an experiment that used objects defined by color and shape. In different change detection task blocks, where memory set size (2 vs. 5 items) varied randomly, participants were to indicate change in either color or shape. ERPs from 12 participants were simultaneously recorded during task performance. A negative amplitude shift over parieto-occipital electrodes, about 200 msec after stimulus onset, was triggered by detected changes, as compared with undetected and no changes. In conclusion, the present study strengthens the view that a posterior ERP negativity provides a neural correlate of processes associated with changes in visual awareness. (5010) Repetition Blindness for Chinese Characters: Form and Meaning Matter. CAILI WU, ALBRECHT W. INHOFF, & CYNTHIA M. CONNINE, SUNY, Binghamton (sponsored by Albrecht W. Inhoff)— Repetition blindness (RB) is the failure to detect the second occurrence of repeated words presented in rapid serial visual presentation. However, absolute repetition of two items is not the prerequisite for RB. RB was found for two English words that were either visually or phonologically similar. Yet there was little evidence for a semantic contribution to RB. By using some unique properties of Chinese characters, two experiments were conducted to investigate whether the form, sound, and meaning of a Chinese character contributes to the RB effect. The results showed form overlap alone was sufficient to produce RB but that sound overlap alone was not adequate for RB to occur. Given some form overlap (i.e., two characters shared the same semantic radical), semantically related character pairs yielded more RB than did semantically unrelated character pairs. (5011) Object Substitution Masking Interferes With Semantic Processing: Evidence From ERPs. JASON E. REISS & JAMES E. HOFFMAN, University of Delaware—Object substitution masking (OSM) refers to impaired target identification caused by the common onset, but delayed offset, of a surrounding dot mask. This effect is believed to result from reentrant processes that replace the target representation with the mask. However, little is known about the degree of processing associated with masked targets. We investigated this issue by examining the effect of OSM on the N400 ERP component, which reflects the degree of semantic mismatch between a target and its context. Participants read a “context” word followed by a semantically related or unrelated “target” word surrounded by dots. As was expected, delayed dot offset led to significantly reduced target identification accuracy. Importantly, the N400 component was also diminished by OSM. These results indicate that OSM interferes with target processing prior to semantic analysis, demonstrating an important difference between OSM and visual phenomena such as the “attentional blink,” where semantic processing is independent of identification accuracy. (5012) Temporal Dynamics of Suppression in the Distractor Previewing Effect. ALEJANDRO LLERAS, University of Illinois, Urbana- Champaign, & JUN KAWAHARA, Hiroshima University—When a visual search for a color oddball (e.g., a red diamond among green diamonds) is performed, oddball-absent trials (for which no response is required) affect performance on the subsequent oddball-present trial, an effect known as the distractor previewing effect (DPE). Specifi- 125 cally, the color of the distractors in the oddball-absent trial (e.g., green) suppresses responses to a target of that color in the subsequent trial (a green target among red distractors), even though participants report a target attribute uncorrelated with color. We investigated the temporal dynamics of this suppression. On oddball-present trials, we switched the color of the items at different points during the trial. Oddball status and correct response were maintained before and after the switch. Despite this, we found that color-switches occurring as early as 100 msec into the trial reduced the magnitude of the DPE by about 50%. Implications regarding the locus of the DPE are discussed. (5013) Speed–Accuracy Tradeoff in Reaction Times During Smooth Pursuit. YASUHIRO SEYA & SHUJI MORI, Tokyo Metropolitan University— We examined reaction times (RTs) during smooth pursuit eye movement with different retinal eccentricities and stimulus velocities. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to pursue a row of circular frames moving at a constant velocity and to respond to the target presented within one of the frames. In Experiment 2, the participants were presented with stimuli identical to those used in Experiment 1 while fixating their eyes on stationary central points. The results of Experiments 1 and 2 showed that the RTs during both smooth pursuit and fixation increased with the retinal eccentricity. The RTs during fixation increased with the stimulus velocity, whereas RTs during smooth pursuit did not change systematically with velocity (although there were large individual differences). We propose that the latter finding is due to a tradeoff between response speed (RT) and pursuit accuracy. • ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING AND SKILL ACQUISITION • (5014) Imagine That! Motor Imagery Enhances Repetition Priming of Sequences. ERICA L. WOHLDMANN, ALICE F. HEALY, & LYLE E. BOURNE, University of Colorado, Boulder—We examined the efficacy of motor imagery for sequence learning and skill acquisition. Subjects participated in four experimental phases: familiarization, training, testing, and recognition. Familiarization involved typing 32 four-digit numbers. During training, 64 different four-digit numbers were presented five times each. The 80 subjects were assigned to four training groups: The control group merely looked at each number; the physical group typed each number; the imagery-move group imagined typing each number while making finger movements; and the imagery-grip group imagined typing each number while holding a mouse to inhibit finger movements. At testing, which required typing old and new numbers, all but the control group showed repetition priming (i.e., an old–new difference). The recognition test indicated that explicit memory was neither necessary nor sufficient for repetition priming. These findings suggest that motor imagery can be used to learn new sequence representations, but not necessarily new motor skills. (5015) Observed Individual Differences by Controlling Any Timings of Occurrences of Vast Study Events. TAKAFUMI TERASAWA, Okayama University, TETSUYA YOSHIDA, Tokoha Gakuen University, KYOKO MAEMOTO, Okayama Higashi Commercial High School, ATSUSHI KATSUBE, Okayama University, & NOBUO OHTA, Tokyo University of Social Welfare—We conducted a 5-week learning study on the second-language (English) acquisition of high school students. At a study phase, students were asked to rate their achievement (i.e., how for they had mastered the Japanese meaning of each English word) for each English–Japanese pair word on a 4-point scale (0 = no good at all, 1 = no good, 2 = good, and 3 = perfect). For all the pairs, the number of study repetitions (1, 4) and occurrences of learning events were controlled. For controlling the timing of occurrences of vast study events (over 1 million), a new experimental design had to be developed. The design also enables researchers to measure the ef-

Saturday Evening Posters 5009–5015<br />

fordances change with changes in body shape (e.g., becoming more<br />

accurate while a well-stuffed backpack is worn).<br />

(5009)<br />

Changes in Visual Awareness: Event-Related Brain Potential Correlates.<br />

HARTMUT LEUTHOLD, University of Glasgow—In a recent<br />

event-related brain potential (ERP) study (Koivistu & Revonsuo, 2003),<br />

awareness of visual change has been associated with an early negative<br />

ERP deflection over posterior electrodes. This study employed<br />

rectangular stimuli that varied only in orientation. Thus, to investigate<br />

whether the change-related ERP negativity is independent of the object<br />

dimension changing, I conducted an experiment that used objects<br />

defined by color and shape. In different change detection task blocks,<br />

where memory set size (2 vs. 5 items) varied randomly, participants<br />

were to indicate change in either color or shape. ERPs from 12 participants<br />

were simultaneously recorded during task performance. A<br />

negative amplitude shift over parieto-occipital electrodes, about<br />

200 msec after stimulus onset, was triggered by detected changes, as<br />

compared with undetected and no changes. In conclusion, the present<br />

study strengthens the view that a posterior ERP negativity provides a<br />

neural correlate of processes associated with changes in visual awareness.<br />

(5010)<br />

Repetition Blindness for Chinese Characters: Form and Meaning<br />

Matter. CAILI WU, ALBRECHT W. INHOFF, & CYNTHIA M.<br />

CONNINE, SUNY, Binghamton (sponsored by Albrecht W. Inhoff)—<br />

Repetition blindness (RB) is the failure to detect the second occurrence<br />

of repeated words presented in rapid serial visual presentation.<br />

However, absolute repetition of two items is not the prerequisite for<br />

RB. RB was found for two English words that were either visually or<br />

phonologically similar. Yet there was little evidence for a semantic<br />

contribution to RB. By using some unique properties of Chinese characters,<br />

two experiments were conducted to investigate whether the<br />

form, sound, and meaning of a Chinese character contributes to the<br />

RB effect. <strong>The</strong> results showed form overlap alone was sufficient to<br />

produce RB but that sound overlap alone was not adequate for RB to<br />

occur. Given some form overlap (i.e., two characters shared the same<br />

semantic radical), semantically related character pairs yielded more<br />

RB than did semantically unrelated character pairs.<br />

(5011)<br />

Object Substitution Masking Interferes With Semantic Processing:<br />

Evidence From ERPs. JASON E. REISS & JAMES E. HOFFMAN,<br />

University of Delaware—Object substitution masking (OSM) refers to<br />

impaired target identification caused by the common onset, but delayed<br />

offset, of a surrounding dot mask. This effect is believed to result<br />

from reentrant processes that replace the target representation<br />

with the mask. However, little is known about the degree of processing<br />

associated with masked targets. We investigated this issue by examining<br />

the effect of OSM on the N400 ERP component, which reflects<br />

the degree of semantic mismatch between a target and its<br />

context. Participants read a “context” word followed by a semantically<br />

related or unrelated “target” word surrounded by dots. As was expected,<br />

delayed dot offset led to significantly reduced target identification accuracy.<br />

Importantly, the N400 component was also diminished by<br />

OSM. <strong>The</strong>se results indicate that OSM interferes with target processing<br />

prior to semantic analysis, demonstrating an important difference<br />

between OSM and visual phenomena such as the “attentional blink,”<br />

where semantic processing is independent of identification accuracy.<br />

(5012)<br />

Temporal Dynamics of Suppression in the Distractor Previewing<br />

Effect. ALEJANDRO LLERAS, University of Illinois, Urbana-<br />

Champaign, & JUN KAWAHARA, Hiroshima University—When a<br />

visual search for a color oddball (e.g., a red diamond among green diamonds)<br />

is performed, oddball-absent trials (for which no response is<br />

required) affect performance on the subsequent oddball-present trial,<br />

an effect known as the distractor previewing effect (DPE). Specifi-<br />

125<br />

cally, the color of the distractors in the oddball-absent trial (e.g.,<br />

green) suppresses responses to a target of that color in the subsequent<br />

trial (a green target among red distractors), even though participants<br />

report a target attribute uncorrelated with color. We investigated the<br />

temporal dynamics of this suppression. On oddball-present trials, we<br />

switched the color of the items at different points during the trial.<br />

Oddball status and correct response were maintained before and after<br />

the switch. Despite this, we found that color-switches occurring as<br />

early as 100 msec into the trial reduced the magnitude of the DPE by<br />

about 50%. Implications regarding the locus of the DPE are discussed.<br />

(5013)<br />

Speed–Accuracy Tradeoff in Reaction Times During Smooth Pursuit.<br />

YASUHIRO SEYA & SHUJI MORI, Tokyo Metropolitan University—<br />

We examined reaction times (RTs) during smooth pursuit eye movement<br />

with different retinal eccentricities and stimulus velocities. In<br />

Experiment 1, participants were asked to pursue a row of circular<br />

frames moving at a constant velocity and to respond to the target presented<br />

within one of the frames. In Experiment 2, the participants<br />

were presented with stimuli identical to those used in Experiment 1<br />

while fixating their eyes on stationary central points. <strong>The</strong> results of<br />

Experiments 1 and 2 showed that the RTs during both smooth pursuit<br />

and fixation increased with the retinal eccentricity. <strong>The</strong> RTs during<br />

fixation increased with the stimulus velocity, whereas RTs during<br />

smooth pursuit did not change systematically with velocity (although<br />

there were large individual differences). We propose that the latter<br />

finding is due to a tradeoff between response speed (RT) and pursuit<br />

accuracy.<br />

• ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING AND SKILL ACQUISITION •<br />

(5014)<br />

Imagine That! Motor Imagery Enhances Repetition Priming of<br />

Sequences. ERICA L. WOHLDMANN, ALICE F. HEALY, & LYLE E.<br />

BOURNE, University of Colorado, Boulder—We examined the efficacy<br />

of motor imagery for sequence learning and skill acquisition.<br />

Subjects participated in four experimental phases: familiarization,<br />

training, testing, and recognition. Familiarization involved typing 32<br />

four-digit numbers. During training, 64 different four-digit numbers<br />

were presented five times each. <strong>The</strong> 80 subjects were assigned to four<br />

training groups: <strong>The</strong> control group merely looked at each number; the<br />

physical group typed each number; the imagery-move group imagined<br />

typing each number while making finger movements; and the<br />

imagery-grip group imagined typing each number while holding a<br />

mouse to inhibit finger movements. At testing, which required typing<br />

old and new numbers, all but the control group showed repetition<br />

priming (i.e., an old–new difference). <strong>The</strong> recognition test indicated<br />

that explicit memory was neither necessary nor sufficient for repetition<br />

priming. <strong>The</strong>se findings suggest that motor imagery can be used<br />

to learn new sequence representations, but not necessarily new motor<br />

skills.<br />

(5015)<br />

Observed Individual Differences by Controlling Any Timings of<br />

Occurrences of Vast Study Events. TAKAFUMI TERASAWA, Okayama<br />

University, TETSUYA YOSHIDA, Tokoha Gakuen University,<br />

KYOKO MAEMOTO, Okayama Higashi Commercial High School,<br />

ATSUSHI KATSUBE, Okayama University, & NOBUO OHTA, Tokyo<br />

University of Social Welfare—We conducted a 5-week learning study<br />

on the second-language (English) acquisition of high school students.<br />

At a study phase, students were asked to rate their achievement (i.e.,<br />

how for they had mastered the Japanese meaning of each English<br />

word) for each English–Japanese pair word on a 4-point scale (0 = no<br />

good at all, 1 = no good, 2 = good, and 3 = perfect). For all the pairs,<br />

the number of study repetitions (1, 4) and occurrences of learning<br />

events were controlled. For controlling the timing of occurrences of<br />

vast study events (over 1 million), a new experimental design had to<br />

be developed. <strong>The</strong> design also enables researchers to measure the ef-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!