29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Saturday Noon Posters 4107–4112<br />

University of Western Ontario—<strong>The</strong> triad classification task presents<br />

participants with a target concept and two possible associates. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

associates are related to the target differently, allowing the researcher to<br />

contrast two classification alternatives. Participants choose which alternative<br />

to pair with the target concept. This task has been used to demonstrate<br />

many conceptual and perceptual preferences, including whether<br />

adults might prefer taxonomic groupings, such as bed and crib (Smiley<br />

& Brown, 1979) or thematic groupings, such as baby and crib (Lin &<br />

Murphy, 2001). We explore this issue further by examining the mode of<br />

stimulus presentation (words or pictures) and the sex of participants.<br />

Several experiments showed an overall preference for thematic classifications,<br />

but this was attenuated when the stimuli were presented as pictures.<br />

In particular, male participants tended to make taxonomic classifications<br />

when presented with pictures and thematic classifications when<br />

presented with words. We argue that classification preferences are dynamic<br />

and are influenced by various stimulus properties.<br />

(4107)<br />

Modeling Categorization Response Times Using Decision Trees: A<br />

Goodness-of-Fit Comparison of Rule-Based and Exemplar Models.<br />

DANIEL LAFOND & YVES LACOUTURE, Université Laval (sponsored<br />

by A. A. J. Marley)—Lamberts (2000) has modeled individual<br />

categorization response data from 10 participants, using the extended<br />

generalized context model and the exemplar-based random walk<br />

model. In the present study, we used three rule-based classification<br />

models proposed by Trabasso, Rollins, and Shaughnessy (1971) to fit<br />

the same data. A decision tree consistent with the observed response<br />

choices was first derived for each participant. <strong>The</strong> sequence of decisions<br />

within each tree provides the basis for response time modeling.<br />

Minimizing SSE with a quasi-Newton search algorithm allowed us to<br />

estimate individual parameters. Two goodness-of-fit indicators were<br />

used, MS e and R 2 . Results show that one of the rule-based models,<br />

which has fewer free parameters than both exemplar models, provides<br />

overall best fit. Some implications for modeling categorization<br />

processes are discussed.<br />

(4108)<br />

Exemplar Models Seen as a Mixture of Serial Decision Rules. FABIEN<br />

MATHY, Rutgers University, Piscataway (sponsored by Jacob Feldman)—This<br />

poster reports a study of a decision tree model in Boolean<br />

concept learning. <strong>The</strong> model aims to determine the minimal decision<br />

tree corresponding to a concept. <strong>The</strong> time needed to classify a stimulus<br />

(i.e., response time) is linked to the number of decisions that must<br />

be made in order to know its category. When mixing all equivalent decision<br />

trees for a given concept, the mean predicted response times are<br />

perfectly correlated with those given by exemplar models. We present<br />

an experiment including 84 subjects who learned and applied<br />

Shepard’s (1961) Type IV concept. <strong>The</strong> mean response times fit both<br />

decision tree and exemplar models. However, when the individual patterns<br />

are looked at, the decision tree model explains the results better<br />

than does the exemplar model in 69 out of 84 subjects.<br />

(4109)<br />

Knowledge Partitioning in Categorization Without Context. LEE-<br />

XIENG YANG, National Chung Cheng University, & STEPHAN<br />

LEWANDOWSKY, University of Western Australia (sponsored by<br />

Stephan Lewandowsky)—Knowledge partitioning refers to the notion<br />

that knowledge can be held in independent and nonoverlapping<br />

parcels. People use a context cue to choose a knowledge parcel for responding,<br />

while ignoring information held in other parcels. A number<br />

of studies have reported evidence for such context-gated knowledge<br />

partitioning in function learning (Lewandowsky, Kalish, &<br />

Ngang, 2002) and categorization (Yang & Lewandowsky, 2003,<br />

2004). In addition, knowledge partitioning has been observed in function<br />

learning in the absence of context cues (Kalish, Lewandowsky, &<br />

Kruschke, 2004). We report a study that examined whether knowledge<br />

partitioning can occur without context cues in categorization. Partic-<br />

121<br />

ipants were trained with separate clusters of stimuli located in different<br />

regions of the category space and were found to learn two partial<br />

rules for categorization, as is predicted by the knowledge-partitioning<br />

framework. We show by simulation that a pure exemplar-based model,<br />

such as the GCM, has difficulty accommodating these results.<br />

(4110)<br />

QMQ Shows Male Adolescent Sex Offenders Can Respond to <strong>The</strong>rapy.<br />

R. CHRIS MARTIN, University of Missouri, Kansas City, DEBBIE<br />

K. MARTIN, Quality Motivation Corporation, BONNIE E. POOLE<br />

& LEAH K. GENSHEIMER, University of Missouri, Kansas City, &<br />

SCOTT U. LEGRAND, Devereaux Florida Network—Adolescent male<br />

sex offenders are recalcitrant therapy subjects for many reasons. A<br />

midwestern Boy’s Town facility provided a structured environment<br />

with many group sessions and several individual sessions weekly, with<br />

an emphasis upon behavioral change. <strong>The</strong> Quality of Motivation<br />

Questionnaire (c) is a true/false, computer-scored instrument that provides<br />

38 scales, including amounts of positive and negative motivation,<br />

energy level, emotional stability, depression, life styles, and<br />

skills. Upon presentation, they were deceitful, depressed, and high in<br />

maladaptive skills, such as self-sabotage, self-punishment, and a lack<br />

of problem-solving abilities. Success of treatment was shown in statistically<br />

significant changes in five areas: emotional stability, depression,<br />

decision making, responsibility, and problem solving. Recommendations<br />

are made regarding how to further improve adaptive<br />

skills and reduction in the maladaptive ones. Depression scores, although<br />

reduced, were still high, and specific changes are needed. <strong>The</strong><br />

attitude that sex offenders are immutable might need to change.<br />

(4111)<br />

Accessibility of Taxonomic and Script Knowledge in the Domain of<br />

Food. ANNA Z. VITKIN, JOHN D. COLEY, & KONSTANTIN FEIGIN,<br />

Northeastern University—Ross and Murphy (1999) showed that priming<br />

improved the accessibility of script knowledge about food (apple<br />

as snack), but not taxonomic knowledge (apple as fruit), relative to<br />

neutral priming, suggesting that taxonomic knowledge is more accessible<br />

to reasoning about food. We examined the effects of cross<br />

priming, asking whether incongruent priming would differentially interfere<br />

with accessibility of script versus taxonomic knowledge. Participants<br />

read a sentence about a food that primed either taxonomic or<br />

script knowledge. <strong>The</strong>y then verified the food’s taxonomic or script<br />

category membership (congruent or incongruent with priming). Overall<br />

taxonomic decisions were faster and more accurate than script decisions.<br />

Incongruent priming had no effect on taxonomic decisions,<br />

whereas script decisions were significantly slower and less accurate<br />

for incongruent primes. Taken together with Ross and Murphy (1999),<br />

this suggests that not only is script knowledge less accessible overall,<br />

but also it is actively inhibited by the activation of taxonomic knowledge.<br />

(4112)<br />

Who Do You Look Like? Further Exploration Into the Relationship<br />

Between Names and Faces. MELISSA A. LEA, Union College, ROBIN<br />

D. THOMAS, AARON T. BELL, NATHAN A. LAMKIN, & JEFF H.<br />

SHOLL, Miami University—Many cognitive researchers support the<br />

notion that names and facial features have no relationship with each<br />

other (Bruce & Young, 1986; Burton & Bruce, 1992). However, Lea<br />

(2002) showed that when participants are given a name and are asked<br />

to provide a description of that person by rating second-order relational<br />

features, they give similar descriptions of how that face should<br />

look. For example, the name “Bob” provoked a description of a widefaced,<br />

round-featured man, and “Tim” a thin-faced, thin-featured<br />

man. Conversely, the present study was conducted to determine<br />

whether participants would consistently label a set of prototype faces<br />

with those names. That is, would participants give the wide-faced,<br />

round-featured face the name “Bob”? <strong>The</strong> results demonstrate that<br />

participants are reliable at giving names to prototype faces and support<br />

the idea that faces and names can have a direct relationship.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!