29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 4071–4078 Saturday Noon<br />

of de Fockert et al., if a nontarget famous face matched the target<br />

name of that celebrity on the subsequent trial, a significant negative<br />

priming effect was obtained, irrespective of whether the concurrent<br />

memory load was low or high.<br />

(4071)<br />

Strategy Effects Counteract Distractor Inhibition: Negative Priming<br />

With Constantly Absent Probe Distractors. CHRISTIAN FRINGS,<br />

DIRK WENTURA, & HUBERT D. ZIMMER, Saarland University—<br />

It is an accepted, albeit puzzling, finding that negative priming (NP)<br />

hinges on the presence of distractors in probe displays. In three experiments<br />

without probe distractors, we found evidence that responsebiasing<br />

processes based on the contingency between prime and probe<br />

displays may have caused this finding. It is argued that it is of help,<br />

in standard NP experiments, to process the prime distractor for<br />

preparing the response to the probe target. If this contingency was removed<br />

(Experiments 2 and 3), NP without probe distractors was reliably<br />

observed, whereas no NP emerged if the design comprised the<br />

typical contingency (Experiment 1). Hence, the data suggest that the<br />

absence of NP, which is usually observed under these conditions, may<br />

have been due to a contingency-based component.<br />

(4072)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Time Course of Visual Attention Triggered by Temporal Expectation.<br />

KEN KIHARA & NAOYUKI OSAKA, Kyoto University—<strong>The</strong><br />

purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis that an expectation of<br />

a target onset triggers visual attention before its appearance. Using an<br />

attentional blink paradigm, we investigated whether processing of a<br />

following target (T2) interferes with that of a preceding target (T1)<br />

when the time of T2 onset can be expected. Experiment 1, in which<br />

T1 and T2 tasks were different, did not show the interference. Experiment<br />

2, however, in which both tasks were the same, showed the interference<br />

within 280 msec before T2 onset. Experiment 3 indicated<br />

that even if subjects learned the timing of T2 onset only implicitly, the<br />

interference occurred. In Experiment 4, the processing of an auditory<br />

T1 was not interfered with by a visual T2. <strong>The</strong>se results suggest that<br />

the expectation of target onset triggers attentional processing in a<br />

task- and modality-specific manner about 300 msec prior to target<br />

onset.<br />

(4073)<br />

Age Effects on Task-Switching Over the Telephone. PATRICIA A.<br />

TUN & MARGIE E. LACHMAN, Brandeis University—We have developed<br />

a version of a task-switching test that can be administered<br />

over the telephone, allowing for testing of a wide range of participants,<br />

including those who are unable or unwilling to come into the laboratory.<br />

Data are presented from a large sample of over 900 adults ranging<br />

in age from 30 to 85 years. In accord with testing by computer in<br />

the lab, response latencies reveal significant age-related increases in<br />

reaction time for task-homogeneous blocks of baseline trials, for taskheterogeneous<br />

blocks that require task-switching, and for task-switching<br />

costs.<br />

(4074)<br />

Spatial Attention Can Enhance or Impair Visual Temporal Resolution.<br />

JONGSOO BAEK, KEETAEK KHAM, & MIN-SHIK KIM,<br />

Yonsei University—Transient attention impairs observers’ temporal<br />

resolution at a cued location. This detrimental effect of attention was<br />

ascribed to inhibitory connections from parvocellular to magnocellular<br />

neurons (Yeshurun & Levy, 2003). Alternatively, the difficulty<br />

might arise because attention facilitates the temporal summation of<br />

two successive stimuli. <strong>The</strong> present study examined this hypothesis by<br />

manipulating the luminance polarity of the stimuli against a background.<br />

Attention should not modulate temporal summation of two<br />

antipolar stimuli, because these are processed in separate channels. Indeed,<br />

observers judged the temporal order of two successive stimuli<br />

better for the cued location than for the uncued location when the<br />

stimuli were opposite in polarity, but temporal resolution was worse<br />

116<br />

for the cued location when the stimuli had the same polarity. Thus, attentional<br />

effects on temporal resolution may be attributed to temporal<br />

summation, rather than to parvocellular inhibition of magnocellular<br />

activity.<br />

(4075)<br />

Perceptual Load Effects Inside and Outside the Focus of Attention.<br />

DONALD J. TELLINGHUISEN, Calvin College—Studies (e.g., Lavie<br />

& Cox, 1997) have shown that one irrelevant peripheral distractor presented<br />

in a response competition paradigm could be ignored if a letter–<br />

circle search array contained perceptually similar nontargets (high<br />

perceptual load), but not if it contained dissimilar nontargets (low perceptual<br />

load). I investigated the effects of two distractors, either response<br />

compatible or response incompatible, presented either outside<br />

the search array (one each to the left and right of the array) or inside<br />

the search array (one each to the left and right of a central fixation).<br />

Reaction time data indicated minimal effects of response-incompatible<br />

distractors presented outside, regardless of perceptual load. Responseincompatible<br />

distractors presented inside, however, yielded large interference<br />

effects for low load searches, but not for high load searches.<br />

Thus, two peripheral distractors were more readily ignored than one,<br />

regardless of perceptual load. In addition, perceptual load modulated<br />

the ability to ignore information, even within the focus of attention.<br />

(4076)<br />

Bottom-Up Priming of Top-Down Attentional Control Settings.<br />

CHARLES L. FOLK, Villanova University, & ROGER W. REMING-<br />

TON, Johns Hopkins University—According to the contingent attentional<br />

capture hypothesis, the degree to which a stimulus produces involuntary<br />

shifts of spatial attention depends on whether the defining<br />

properties of the stimulus match top-down attentional control settings.<br />

It has been assumed that these control settings reflect the voluntary<br />

establishment and maintenance of a target “template” based on task<br />

instructions. Here, we show that attentional control settings can be influenced<br />

by stimulus-driven trial-to-trial priming. Subjects searched<br />

for red or green color-singleton targets preceded by uninformative red<br />

or green color-singleton cues. Although all cues produced evidence<br />

of attentional capture (i.e., cuing effects), the magnitude of capture<br />

was significantly greater when the color of the cue on trial n matched<br />

the color of the target on trial n�1. <strong>The</strong>se intertrial priming effects<br />

suggest a stimulus-driven component to the establishment of topdown<br />

attentional control settings.<br />

(4077)<br />

To See or Not to See: Attention Leads to Selection. ZHE CHEN, University<br />

of Canterbury, & KYLE R. CAVE, University of Massachusetts,<br />

Amherst—Object-based attention assumes that attending to an object<br />

results in its selection, such that all its feature dimensions are<br />

processed regardless of whether they are relevant or not. This view<br />

was recently challenged by the finding that the extent of processing<br />

of an irrelevant dimension depends on the status of the attended object.<br />

When it is a target, the irrelevant dimensions are encoded. However,<br />

when it is a distractor, only the relevant dimension is processed.<br />

In several experiments, we employed a singleton paradigm to investigate<br />

the relationship between attention and selection. Our results show<br />

that attention and selection cannot be dissociated. <strong>The</strong> irrelevant dimension<br />

of the singleton distractor was processed even when the location<br />

of the target was invariant. We suggest that the discrepancies<br />

between our results and those of prior research can be resolved by the<br />

nature of the specific feature dimension that receives attention.<br />

(4078)<br />

Letter Processing in the Absence of Attention. JOEL LACHTER,<br />

NASA Ames Research Center, & ERIC RUTHRUFF, University of New<br />

Mexico—One of the most central, and most hotly debated, issues in<br />

attention research concerns the depth to which unattended objects are<br />

processed. Numerous studies have shown that irrelevant flanking<br />

stimuli speed responses to compatible targets and slow responses to

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!