29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 4057–4063 Saturday Noon<br />

(4057)<br />

Feature Binding Within Visuospatial Short-Term Memory: Asymmetry<br />

Between Shape and Location Encoding. JANE ELSLEY &<br />

FABRICE B. PARMENTIER, University of Plymouth (sponsored by Fabrice<br />

B. Parmentier)—Binding processes play an important role in memory.<br />

Using a probe recognition task (Prabhakaran et al., 2000), we investigated<br />

visual-to-spatial binding by presenting three irregular shapes<br />

simultaneously in irregularly distributed locations, followed by a single<br />

probe. Participants had to judge the probe in terms of both the visual<br />

and the spatial features. In Experiment 1, robust binding effects<br />

were observed. Two further experiments investigated whether binding<br />

processes operate automatically. Results indicated that when participants<br />

focused only on shapes (Experiment 2A), binding occurred automatically.<br />

However, when participants focused on locations (Experiment 2B),<br />

no binding effect was observed. <strong>The</strong>refore, shape processing was not<br />

independent of the processing of location, whereas location could be<br />

attended to in isolation. This asymmetry in association is discussed<br />

with regard to recent perceptual and attentional accounts of binding.<br />

(4058)<br />

Training on Relations Eliminates Interference of Elements on Relational<br />

Processing. AARON S. YARLAS, Grand Valley State University,<br />

& VLADIMIR M. SLOUTSKY, Ohio State University—Previous studies<br />

(Yarlas & Sloutsky, 2004) incorporating a Garner interference task<br />

with undergraduates showed clear evidence for asymmetry of interference<br />

when elements and relations were processed: Status of elements<br />

(i.e., objects in an arrangement) could not be ignored during relational<br />

processing, whereas status of relations (i.e., patterns of<br />

arrangements among objects) had no impact on the processing of elements.<br />

<strong>The</strong> present study, in which the same interference task was<br />

preceded by either training focusing on relations (e.g., labeling relations,<br />

sorting by relations) or control training, yielded a much different<br />

outcome, such that after relational training (although not after control<br />

training), the interference of elements with relations disappeared,<br />

and there was, in fact, an emergence of interference of relations with<br />

elements. <strong>The</strong>se findings provide strong evidence that distinctions between<br />

element and relational processing are not fixed a priori but,<br />

rather, are flexible and are likely a product of learning and/or directed<br />

attention.<br />

(4059)<br />

Architecture and Retrieval Dynamics of Working Memory: Exploring<br />

the Role of Pointer Predictability in Redirecting Attention.<br />

CHANDRAMALLIKA BASAK & PAUL VERHAEGHEN, Syracuse<br />

University—Working memory consists of a focus of attention and an<br />

active outer store (Cowan, 1995). We conducted a series of four experiments<br />

to explore the differential capacity and retrieval dynamics<br />

of these two subsystems of working memory and whether a passive<br />

memory load affects the retrieval speed or activation of these subsystems.<br />

We found that when the retrieval sequence is predictable, the<br />

focus switch cost was additive over increasing set size, but when it is<br />

unpredictable, a controlled search slope of about 230 msec/set-size<br />

emerges. Moreover, with extended practice, the focus can be expanded<br />

from one to four in a predictable-sequence task, but not in an<br />

unpredictable-sequence task. We propose that a pointer is needed to<br />

redirect attention to the correct cell in the memory array; resource demands<br />

on a task may depend upon whether the pointer moves predictably<br />

or not.<br />

(4060)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role of Attention During Retrieval in Working Memory Span:<br />

A Dual-Task Study. M. KARL HEALEY, University of Toronto, &<br />

AKIRA MIYAKE, University of Colorado, Boulder—We tested the<br />

hypothesis that operation span (OS) performance involves attentiondemanding<br />

retrieval of target words from long-term memory (LTM),<br />

rather than active maintenance of all target words. Participants completed<br />

the standard OS task and a modified, word-span-like version<br />

114<br />

in which all equations preceded all target words. Recall took place<br />

under either full attention or divided attention conditions (easy vs. difficult).<br />

Secondary-task performance was worse for the standard OS<br />

task than for the modified version, with the difficult secondary task<br />

showing the larger decrement. Recall also suffered under divided attention,<br />

with the recall decrement greater for the more difficult secondary<br />

task. Moreover, the time taken to start recalling the first word<br />

was considerably longer for the standard version than for the modified<br />

version. <strong>The</strong>se results are consistent with the proposal that successful<br />

OS performance involves, in part, the attention-demanding use<br />

of retrieval cues to recover target items from LTM.<br />

(4061)<br />

Comparison of Standardized Measures of Working Memory Across<br />

the Life Span. SANDRA HALE, LISA J. EMERY, JOEL MYERSON,<br />

DESIREE A. WHITE, & DUNEESHA RANATUNGA, Washington<br />

University—Analyses of children’s normative working memory data<br />

from the WISC III revealed parallel developmental changes in forward<br />

and backward digit span, extending our previous analyses of the<br />

WAIS III. Notably, the difference between forward and backward span<br />

remains approximately constant across the life span. Comparison of<br />

performance on the Digit Span and Letter–Number Sequencing subtests<br />

revealed a different pattern. Performance on the two subtests develops<br />

in tandem from the age of 8 years through young adulthood;<br />

scores diverge from middle-age on, however, with performance on<br />

digit span tasks showing a much slower decline than that on<br />

letter–number sequencing tasks. Taken together, the present findings<br />

suggest that at least two mechanisms are involved in age-related differences<br />

in working memory. Our findings pose problems, however,<br />

for the view that one of these mechanisms, distinct from short-term<br />

memory and associated with the frontal lobes, develops more slowly<br />

in childhood and declines more rapidly in later adulthood.<br />

(4062)<br />

Do Phonological Processes Contribute to Aging Effects on Verbal<br />

STM? DANIEL KLEINMAN & DEBORAH M. BURKE, Pomona College<br />

(sponsored by Deborah M. Burke)—Age deficits in phonological<br />

activation processes have been implicated in word production failures<br />

in old age. We tested this phonological deficit hypothesis by examining<br />

the effect of phonological neighborhood density of words on<br />

short-term memory (STM) in young and older adults. Words with<br />

dense neighborhoods share phonemes with many other words; this<br />

benefits STM because interactive excitatory processes among phonologically<br />

similar words maintain activation of words in STM (e.g.,<br />

Roodenrys, Hulme, Lethbridge, Hinton, & Nimmo, 2002). Young and<br />

older adults recalled word lists varying in word frequency of occurrence<br />

and neighborhood density, in a memory span procedure. Frequency<br />

effects were the same across age, but high neighborhood density<br />

improved STM more for young than for older adults. Implications<br />

for the processes underlying frequency, neighborhood, and aging effects<br />

in STM are discussed.<br />

• ATTENTION •<br />

(4063)<br />

Attentional Modulation of the Amygdala Varies With Personality.<br />

STEVEN B. MOST & MARVIN M. CHUN, Yale University, &<br />

MATTHEW R. JOHNSON & KENT A. KIEHL, Institute of Living,<br />

Hartford Hospital—Prominent theories suggest that the amygdala responds<br />

to emotional stimuli independently of attention. In contrast, we<br />

found evidence for attentional modulation of the amygdala, but this<br />

varied as a function of personality. We employed an attentional task<br />

where participants always had to ignore emotional and neutral distractors<br />

while searching for a target. <strong>The</strong> ease of ignoring distractors<br />

was manipulated through the degree of specificity with which targets<br />

were described to participants; more specific information about targets<br />

generally leads to better ignoring of distractors (Most et al., in

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!