29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Saturday Noon Posters 4051–4056<br />

lists of words, one “read” (i.e., whole words) and the other “selfgenerated”<br />

(word fragment completion). Each block was followed by<br />

a recognition test. Generating studied items increased the tendency for<br />

7- to 8-year-old children to recollect those items, as compared with<br />

having read them. In contrast, 10- to 11-year-old children relied more<br />

heavily on graded similarity judgments when accepting studied items<br />

that had been generated, in comparison with studied items that were<br />

read. <strong>The</strong>se findings are consistent with a fuzzy-trace theory account<br />

of memory development.<br />

(4051)<br />

Role of Experimental Design and Age on the Generation Effect.<br />

LAURENCE TACONNAT, MATHILDE SACHER, DAVID CLARYS,<br />

SANDRINE VANNESTE, & MICHEL ISINGRINI, Université de Tours<br />

(sponsored by Naftali Raz)—<strong>The</strong> generation effect (e.g., generated<br />

words are better recalled than read words: wall-hou__ (vs.) wallhouse)<br />

was investigated in between-list and mixed-list designs in<br />

young and elderly participants. <strong>The</strong> generation difficulty was also manipulated<br />

by varying the strength of association between cues and targets.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results showed different patterns of generation effect according<br />

to the design, difficulty, and age, with a robust generation<br />

effect in the mixed-list design whatever the participants’ age, corroborating<br />

previous researches. In the between-list design, elderly participants<br />

did never benefit from the generation effect, and in young<br />

participants, the effect was elicited only when they generated strong<br />

associates. Findings showed that the type of design did not allow implementation<br />

of the same cognitive operations according to age. Contrary<br />

to the generation effect in a mixed-list design, the generation effect<br />

in a between-list design might be the consequence of effortful<br />

processes, difficult to implement in elderly adults, whereas the generation<br />

effect in a mixed-list design might be more automatic.<br />

• WORKING MEMORY •<br />

(4052)<br />

Effects of Complexity on the Recall of Spatial Configurations.<br />

AYSECAN BODURO ˇGLU & PRITI SHAH, University of Michigan—<br />

Recent research has demonstrated that spatial working memory representations<br />

preserve the overall configuration formed by a number<br />

of attended locations. In a series of studies using the serial spatial recall<br />

task, we investigated how configural complexity influences<br />

(1) the independent recall of locations in a sequence and (2) the recall<br />

of the overall configurations formed by a sequence. Configural complexity<br />

was manipulated by varying the number of path crossings in<br />

a spatial sequence (zero, one, or three). For highly complex sequences,<br />

although complexity did not impact recall of independent locations,<br />

it did impair how well the configurations were preserved, as compared<br />

with simpler sequences. Results indicate that localization errors in<br />

spatial recall tasks are influenced more by length of and serial position<br />

in sequences, whereas orientations of errors are more influenced<br />

by complexity of configurations. <strong>The</strong>se findings highlight the dissociation<br />

between distance and orientation errors for characterizing spatial<br />

recall accuracy.<br />

(4053)<br />

Location-Based Versus Configurational Representations of Visuospatial<br />

Information in Working Memory. LEON GMEINDL &<br />

JAMES K. NELSON, University of Michigan, TIMOTHY D. WIGGIN,<br />

Eastern Michigan University, & JENNIFER C. HSU & PATRICIA A.<br />

REUTER-LORENZ, University of Michigan—Do people represent<br />

multiple spatial targets in working memory (WM) as independent locations<br />

or as configurations? In three delayed location matching-tosample<br />

experiments, participants viewed three dots presented simultaneously<br />

for 1,500 msec, followed 3 sec later by a three-dot probe. For<br />

nonmatch probes, preserved configurations led to false alarms more<br />

frequently than did distorted configurations, despite equivalent stimulus<br />

displacements. Furthermore, a higher false alarm rate for pre-<br />

113<br />

served configurations persisted when only a single probe stimulus was<br />

designated as relevant by its unique color. In contrast, an arrow cue presented<br />

prior to target presentation eliminated the increased false alarm<br />

rate for probes with preserved configurations. We conclude that<br />

(1) people utilize configurational information in WM representations<br />

of multiple locations and (2) attentional processes can reduce the influence<br />

of configurational information on WM representations.<br />

(4054)<br />

Is the Binding of Visual Features in Working Memory Resource-<br />

Demanding? RICHARD J. ALLEN, ALAN D. BADDELEY, & GRA-<br />

HAM J. HITCH, University of York (sponsored by Alan D. Baddeley)—<br />

A series of experiments examined the processes underlying the encoding<br />

and retention of visual feature bindings in working memory. Memory<br />

for shapes or colors was compared with memory for combinations of<br />

these features. When demanding concurrent verbal tasks were used,<br />

with the intention of disrupting executive resources, visual memory<br />

performance was significantly impaired. However, the effects were no<br />

greater for memory for feature combinations than for the features<br />

themselves. A comparison of simultaneous and sequential item presentations<br />

revealed the combination condition to be significantly<br />

worse in the latter, especially for items earlier in the sequence. <strong>The</strong><br />

findings are interpreted as evidence for a relatively fragile but automatic<br />

visual feature binding mechanism in working memory, capable<br />

of functioning without placing additional demands on the central executive.<br />

This form of ‘unitized’ binding is then compared with the<br />

binding of spatially separated features.<br />

(4055)<br />

Feature Binding in Visual Working Memory Without Continued<br />

Attention. DANIEL A. GAJEWSKI & JAMES R. BROCKMOLE,<br />

Michigan State University (sponsored by James R. Brockmole)—Are<br />

integrated objects the unit of capacity of visual working memory, or<br />

is continued attention needed to maintain bindings between independently<br />

stored features? In a delayed recall task, participants reported<br />

the color and shape of a probed item from a memory array. During the<br />

delay, attention was manipulated with an exogenous cue. Recall was<br />

elevated at validly cued positions, indicating that the cue affected item<br />

memory. On invalid trials, participants most frequently recalled either<br />

both features (perfect object memory) or neither of the two features<br />

(no object memory); the frequency with which only one feature was<br />

recalled was significantly lower than predicted by feature independence<br />

as determined in a single-feature recall task. <strong>The</strong>se data do not<br />

support the view that features are remembered independently when attention<br />

is withdrawn. Instead, integrated objects are stored in visual<br />

working memory without need for continued attention.<br />

(4056)<br />

Eye Movements Reveal Strategic Interactions Between Perceptual<br />

Attention and Visual Working Memory During Visual Search.<br />

LEANNE BOUCHER & GEOFFREY F. WOODMAN, Vanderbilt<br />

University—<strong>The</strong>ories of attention propose that maintaining a representation<br />

in visual working memory guides perceptual attention to<br />

similar items. However, a recent study showed that when participants<br />

maintained a visual working memory representation and performed<br />

visual search, the presence of a memory-matching distractor that was<br />

never the target did not slow search. Here, we sought to more directly<br />

measure selection by tracking observers’ eye movements. Eye<br />

movement data revealed that search was faster when the memorymatching<br />

distractor was in the array, because observers generally<br />

avoided fixating this nontarget until search was completed. However,<br />

after the search target was fixated and discriminated, subjects directed<br />

gaze to the memory-matching item, consistent with a working memory<br />

maintenance strategy of perceptual resampling. Thus, attention<br />

can be biased toward or away from items that match the contents of<br />

visual working memory, and these biases can dynamically change in<br />

the service of different search and memory maintenance strategies.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!