29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

Abstracts 2005 - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 3098–3104 Friday Evening<br />

also extend recent work on the benefits of multimedia learning (Gyselinck<br />

et al., 2002; Mayer, 2001).<br />

(3098)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role of Conceptual Relations in Recognition Memory. LARA L.<br />

JONES, ZACHARY ESTES, & RICHARD L. MARSH, University of<br />

Georgia—We assessed whether conceptual relations (WOOL BLANKET:<br />

made of ) facilitate recognition memory for the constituent concepts<br />

(WOOL and BLANKET), as suggested by the relational information hypothesis<br />

(Humphreys, 1976). In each experiment, participants judged<br />

the sensicality of word pairs prior to a surprise recognition memory test.<br />

Experiment 1 showed that recognition memory for word pairs was facilitated<br />

by relational information. In Experiment 2, relational information<br />

facilitated memory for the modifier (WOOL), but not for the head<br />

noun (BLANKET), and only when the same relation was instantiated at<br />

study and test. Experiment 3A replicated this result with individual,<br />

rather than paired, presentation of words at test. When the memory test<br />

was not a surprise, in Experiment 3B, conceptual relations did not facilitate<br />

memory. Results support the relational information hypothesis<br />

and suggest that relational information is more strongly associated with<br />

the modifier than with the head noun (Gagne & Shoben, 1997).<br />

• REASONING •<br />

(3099)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role of Interpretation in Causal Learning. CHRISTIAN C.<br />

LUHMANN, Vanderbilt University, & WOO-KYOUNG AHN, Yale<br />

University (sponsored by Woo-Kyoung Ahn)—When learning causal<br />

relations from covariation information, do people give causal interpretations<br />

to covariation as it is encountered, or is covariation simply<br />

recorded verbatim for later causal judgments? Participants in Study 1<br />

performed a causal learning task and were asked to periodically interpret<br />

observations. Participants’ interpretations were context sensitive;<br />

identical observations were interpreted differently when encountered<br />

in different contexts. <strong>The</strong>se interpretations also predicted<br />

their final causal strength judgments. Those who were influenced<br />

more by initial covariation (primacy effect) tended to interpret later<br />

observations as consistent with initial covariation. Conversely, those<br />

who were influenced more by later covariation (recency effect) tended<br />

to interpret later observations as consistent with later covariation.<br />

When participants in Study 2 were required to predict the presence of<br />

the effect on each trial, overall recency effects were obtained, but interpretations<br />

were still context sensitive and predicted causal strength<br />

judgments. We discuss the difficulty these results pose for current<br />

models.<br />

(3100)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Influence of Virtual Sample Size on Confidence in Causal<br />

Judgments. MIMI LILJEHOLM & PATRICIA CHENG, UCLA—We<br />

investigated the extent to which confidence in causal judgments varied<br />

with virtual sample size—the frequency of cases in which the outcome<br />

is (1) absent before the introduction of a generative cause or<br />

(2) present before the introduction of a preventive cause. Participants<br />

were asked to evaluate the influence of various candidate causes on<br />

an outcome, as well as their confidence in judgments about those influences.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y were presented with information on the relative frequencies<br />

of the outcome, given the presence and absence of various<br />

candidate causes. <strong>The</strong> study manipulated these relative frequencies,<br />

sample size, and the direction of the causal influence (generative vs.<br />

preventive). We found that confidence varied with virtual sample size<br />

and that both confidence and causal strength ratings showed a clear,<br />

and previously undocumented, asymmetry between the two causal<br />

directions.<br />

(3101)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Useful Application of a Discounting Heuristic in Causal Inference.<br />

KELLY M. GOEDERT, Seton Hall University, & SARAH A.<br />

LARSON, Pacific Lutheran University—Previously, we demonstrated<br />

102<br />

that people reduce their causal effectiveness ratings of a moderately<br />

effective cause in the presence of a highly effective one (i.e., discounting;<br />

Goedert & Spellman, <strong>2005</strong>). Here, we assess whether participants<br />

recruit this discounting heuristic because it is useful. In positive<br />

outcome situations, finding one cause may be good enough. With<br />

negative outcomes, one must find all possible causes to eliminate an<br />

aversive event. Participants received contingency information about<br />

potential causes of either a positive outcome (medications leading to<br />

allergy relief) or a negative outcome (environmental irritants causing<br />

allergies) over 72 trials. In both situations, a moderately effective<br />

cause was learned about in the presence of an alternative that was either<br />

highly effective or not at all effective. Participants discounted the<br />

moderately effective cause in positive, but not negative, outcome situations.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that participants selectively recruit the<br />

discounting heuristic when finding that only one cause is sufficient.<br />

(3102)<br />

Darwin and Design: Do Evolutionary Explanations Require Understanding<br />

Evolution? TANIA LOMBROZO, Harvard University (sponsored<br />

by Yuhong Jiang)—Three experiments examined the relationship<br />

between knowledge of causal mechanisms and explanatory understanding.<br />

In Experiment 1, 64 participants identified statements that<br />

would falsify a functional explanation for an artifact (a product of<br />

human design) or a biological part (a product of natural selection). Although<br />

participants judged functional explanations for artifacts and<br />

biological parts equally satisfying, they were significantly worse at<br />

identifying statements that falsify evolutionary explanations. Experiments<br />

2 (N = 72) and 3 (N = 36) showed that participants’ judgments<br />

of the goodness of evolutionary explanations are uncorrelated with<br />

their ability to identify statements that falsify the explanation and that<br />

the inability to identify falsifying statements results from a poor understanding<br />

of the mechanisms by which natural selection operates.<br />

<strong>The</strong> feeling of understanding that accompanies an evolutionary explanation<br />

may result from knowledge that an underlying mechanism<br />

that parallels human design exists (namely, natural selection), no matter<br />

that the mechanism is not understood.<br />

(3103)<br />

Priming Category and Analogy Relations During Relational Reasoning.<br />

ADAM E. GREEN, JONATHAN A. FUGELSANG, KEVIN A.<br />

SMITH, & KEVIN N. DUNBAR, Dartmouth College—Green, Fugelsang,<br />

and Dunbar (2004) have demonstrated that the processing of<br />

analogies primes two kinds of underlying relations, category and analogy<br />

relations, and that these primed relations have an effect on subsequent<br />

tasks. In Green et al. (2004), we found that this priming interfered<br />

with performance in a Stroop task. Here, using a naming task and the<br />

same stimuli, we hypothesized that processing of analogies should facilitate<br />

reading of words that refer to category and analogy relations.<br />

Results were consistent with this hypothesis, and we discuss the results<br />

with respect to models of analogical reasoning and priming effects.<br />

(3104)<br />

Cue Competition With Continuous Cues and Outcomes. JASON M.<br />

TANGEN, BEN R. NEWELL, & FRED WESTBROOK, University of<br />

New South Wales (sponsored by Ben R. Newell)—<strong>The</strong> simplest measure<br />

of causal inference is based on the presence or absence of causes<br />

and effects. Traditionally, animal and human contingency learning experiments<br />

have investigated the role of these binary events that do not<br />

capture the nature of most everyday assessment tasks. In a basic twophase<br />

design (Phase 1, A�/B�; Phase 2, AC�/BD�), animals tend<br />

to respond less to C (blocking) than to D (superconditioning). We employ<br />

this design with human participants and use continuous cues and<br />

outcomes, rather than binary events (i.e., different quantities of liquid<br />

resulting in various levels of plant growth). Under these conditions,<br />

we demonstrate that the differential treatment of C and D in the second<br />

phase disappears. Although conditional contingency has been discussed<br />

at length with respect to binary events, we will discuss the role<br />

of its continuous analogue in human covariation assessment.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!