29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Friday Evening Posters 3079–3084<br />

TURA, University of the Saarland (sponsored by Ruth H. Maki)—In<br />

a police lineup, the psychological state of the suspects differs from<br />

that of the foils, in that the suspects face a situation that can potentially<br />

lead to personal loss if they are identified. Weigold and Wentura<br />

(2004) showed that being a suspect can result in detectable behavioral<br />

cues. Mock suspects were randomly selected and were promised<br />

money for not being found by mock eyewitnesses. <strong>The</strong>y were filmed<br />

before (control condition) and after (experimental condition) their selection.<br />

<strong>The</strong> probability of identification of the mock suspects was<br />

significantly higher in the experimental than in the control condition.<br />

It was speculated that eyewitnesses used comparisons between lineup<br />

members to find the member that was “different” from the others.<br />

Since this might be remedied by using a sequential lineup procedure,<br />

the original study was replicated with a simultaneous versus sequential<br />

condition. <strong>The</strong> results indicate that sequential lineups decrease<br />

identification of mock suspects.<br />

• MEMORY FOR FACES AND SCENES •<br />

(3079)<br />

Face Recognition for Own-Age and Own-Race Faces. JEFFREY S.<br />

ANASTASI & JASON L. RANDALL, Sam Houston State University,<br />

& MATTHEW G. RHODES, Colorado State University—Previous<br />

studies have demonstrated that individuals are better at recognizing<br />

in-group compared with out-group faces. We report two experiments<br />

in which participants studied same- and other-race faces that came<br />

from their own or other age groups. Specifically, white and black participants<br />

were shown white and black faces that were from the same<br />

or different age range as the participants. Participants exhibited superior<br />

memory for faces from their own race compared with otherrace<br />

faces (i.e., an own-race bias). Additionally, participants exhibited<br />

superior recognition of faces from their own age group (i.e., own-age<br />

bias) but only for faces from their own race. No such own-age bias was<br />

apparent for other-race faces. <strong>The</strong> results are discussed with regard to<br />

Sporer’s in-group/out-group model (IOM).<br />

(3080)<br />

Computer-Generated Faces Are Not Processed Like Real Faces.<br />

CURT A. CARLSON & SCOTT D. GRONLUND, University of Oklahoma—We<br />

conducted three experiments to test whether computergenerated<br />

(CG) faces (FACES 4.0) are processed like real faces, either<br />

perceptually or memorially. Experiments 1 and 2 replicated the<br />

face inversion effect for real faces (Yin, 1969), but inversion had no<br />

effect on accuracy for CG faces. <strong>The</strong> results from Experiment 3<br />

showed that increased encoding time (1 sec vs. 3 sec) had no effect on<br />

recognition memory performance or response time for real faces, but<br />

for the CG faces, accuracy and response time increased as study time<br />

increased. Application of the EZ-Diffusion model (Wagenmakers<br />

et al., 2007) provided insights into processing differences between CG<br />

faces and real faces. CG faces are processed less configurally than are<br />

real faces, which has implications for their use as stimuli in experiments<br />

exploring the cognitive processes underlying various applied<br />

memory phenomena.<br />

(3081)<br />

Influence of Peripheral Distinctive Features: Body Hair and Facial<br />

Recognition. JENNIFER THIBODEAUX & JAMES B. WORTHEN,<br />

Southeastern Louisiana University—<strong>The</strong> influence of a distinctive<br />

noncentral trait on recognition for faces was investigated in a single<br />

experiment. At encoding, participants were presented with a mixed<br />

list of waist-up photographs of women. Half of the photographs depicted<br />

women with visible armpit hair and half depicted the Western<br />

cultural norm of shaved armpits. After a 48-h retention interval, participants<br />

were given an old/new recognition test using photographs of<br />

faces only. <strong>The</strong> results of a multiple regression analysis indicated an<br />

interaction between hirsutism and attractiveness such that faces of attractive<br />

hirsute women were recognized better than faces in all other<br />

conditions. <strong>The</strong> results suggest that distinctiveness of a noncentral<br />

98<br />

feature can enhance memory for target stimuli. <strong>The</strong> relevance of the<br />

results for current theories of distinctiveness is discussed.<br />

(3082)<br />

Individual Differences in Face Processing: Behavioral and Psychophysiological<br />

Indicators. GRIT HERZMANN, OLGA KUNINA,<br />

OLIVER WILHELM, & WERNER SOMMER, Humboldt University,<br />

Berlin (sponsored by Werner Sommer)—Functional and neuroanatomical<br />

models predict, and empirical evidence confirms, that<br />

event-related potentials (ERPs) are associated with distinguishable aspects<br />

of face processing and might serve as indicators of individual<br />

differences in face processing abilities. We investigated the extent to<br />

which such individual differences can be captured in the following<br />

ERP components: N170, difference due to memory, early and late repetition<br />

effects, and the old/new effect. 209 participants underwent intensive<br />

behavioral testing. 86 of them were randomly selected to take<br />

part in two EEG sessions where faces were learned and after 1 week<br />

were tested for recognition. All ERP components exhibited high internal<br />

consistencies, and confirmatory factor analysis showed them to<br />

be unidimensional. Latent factors for the behavioral measures of face<br />

processing correlated moderately with the repetition effects, the<br />

old/new effect, and the N170 latency. Thus, individuals with faster<br />

structural encoding and higher activation during recognition showed<br />

better face recognition performance.<br />

(3083)<br />

Children’s Identification From Lineups Matching Suspects, Descriptions,<br />

or Composites. IN-KYEONG KIM, CLARISSA NAM-<br />

BIAR, ANASTACIA RODRIGUEZ, MARCELLO RUVALCABA,<br />

SAIDA L. SOLIS, & MELISSA VEGA, La Sierra University—This<br />

study investigated the selection of distractors for identification lineups<br />

based on suspect-matched, description-matched, and compositematched<br />

procedures. Composite-matched procedure was newly developed<br />

and had components of visual description. Twelve 9- to<br />

11-year-old children participated in verbally describing the perpetrator<br />

or making facial composites (using Identi-Kit v.6) with mock police<br />

officers immediately after viewing a crime scene video. Lineups<br />

were then produced on the basis of the similarity to the suspect picture,<br />

to the verbal description, and to the composites by 6 mock police<br />

officers. An additional 30 9- to 11-year-old children participated<br />

in the identification of the perpetrator 2 days after viewing the video.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results showed that the identification from the description-matched<br />

lineups was more accurate than the other two in the perpetrator-absent<br />

condition. Participants’ confidence in their answers was higher in the<br />

perpetrator-present condition than in the perpetrator-absent condition.<br />

<strong>The</strong> same experiment with 30 adult participants showed no significant<br />

differences among three lineup procedures.<br />

(3084)<br />

Typicality Congruency of Misinformation. MEHGEN DELANEY,<br />

ROBERT F. BELLI, & AYAKO MASUDA, University of Nebraska,<br />

Lincoln (sponsored by Robert F. Belli)—<strong>The</strong> present study investigated<br />

schema consistency on the suggestibility of memory for realworld<br />

common scenes. Participants were shown still photographs of<br />

scenes, which included either only typical or some atypical items.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y were later exposed to narratives containing additive misinformation<br />

of atypical and typical items, and then were asked to reflect<br />

on their phenomenological experience of remembering by giving remember/know<br />

responses to misinformation items in a cued recall test.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results replicate the findings of Nemeth and Belli (2006) with<br />

participants providing significantly more remember and know responses<br />

to atypical misinformation, but only when shown scenes with<br />

atypical items. When shown typical items, significantly more remember<br />

responses were provided for the typical misinformation<br />

items. <strong>The</strong> results demonstrate a congruent misinformation effect in<br />

that false memories for suggested items, as measured by remember responses,<br />

are more likely to occur when there is a match between the<br />

typicality of shown and suggested items.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!