29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Friday Evening Posters 3064–3070<br />

codes that drive the Simon effect. Our results show that the Simon effect<br />

was determined by the location of the stimulus, regardless of attentional<br />

shift. <strong>The</strong>se results challenge the previously upheld belief<br />

that attention alone mediates Simon effects.<br />

(3064)<br />

Compatibility for Pure and Mixed Mappings of Discrete and Continuous<br />

Stimuli and Responses. MOTONORI YAMAGUCHI &<br />

ROBERT W. PROCTOR, Purdue University—<strong>The</strong> present research examined<br />

effects of perceptual similarity on the magnitude of stimulus–<br />

response compatibility (SRC) effects with mixed mappings. Two experiments<br />

tested the SRC effect with pure and mixed mappings for factorial<br />

pairings of discrete location and continuous rotation stimuli with discrete<br />

buttonpress and continuous yoke-turn responses. <strong>The</strong> results<br />

showed that, with pure mappings, the yoke-turns yielded a larger SRC<br />

effect with the continuous stimuli than with the discrete stimuli, whereas<br />

the buttonpresses yielded a larger SRC effect with the discrete stimuli<br />

than with the continuous stimuli. <strong>The</strong>se outcomes suggest that the degree<br />

of set-level compatibility is higher when discrete-continuous characteristics<br />

of stimulus and response sets match than when they do not.<br />

However, reductions of SRC effects with mixed mappings were little affected<br />

by perceptual similarity between stimulus and response sets.<br />

(30<strong>65</strong>)<br />

Response-Locked LRPs From the Complication Procedure: Evidence<br />

Against Pure Insertion. J. TOBY MORDKOFF, University of<br />

Iowa, LESLIE A. ADERHOLD, Pennsylvania State University, PEGGY<br />

CHEN, Kessler Institute for Rehabilitation, BRIE SULLIVAN &<br />

REBECCA VON DER HEIDE, Pennsylvania State University, & ROSE<br />

HALTERMAN, University of Iowa—When using the complication<br />

procedure, performance is compared across simple-RT, go/no-go, and<br />

forced-choice tasks. In order to interpret the differences in response<br />

time as estimates of the durations of component stages, it must be assumed<br />

that components may be inserted (or deleted) without causing<br />

changes in the remaining components. We tested whether the assumption<br />

of pure insertion applies to late, motor processes by examining<br />

the response-locked, lateralized readiness potential (LRP). We<br />

found evidence against pure insertion, as well as further evidence concerning<br />

the functional significance of the LRP.<br />

(3066)<br />

<strong>The</strong> STOP Signal That Makes the Eyes GO: Evidence for an Exogenous<br />

Effect on Countermanding Saccades. RICHARD GODIJN &<br />

ARTHUR F. KRAMER, Beckman Institute, University of Illinois (sponsored<br />

by Arthur F. Kramer)—Performance in the stop-signal paradigm<br />

is typically described as a race between independent GO and STOP<br />

processes. However, competitive integration models of oculomotor<br />

control predict that a visual stop signal should interact with oculomotor<br />

programming to a target location. In a series of experiments,<br />

participants were instructed to execute an endogenous saccade, but to<br />

stop the saccade if a visual stop signal was presented. To test the idea<br />

that visual stop signals interact with the endogenous oculomotor program,<br />

we manipulated the location of the stop signal. <strong>The</strong> results revealed<br />

that stopping performance was impaired when the stop signal<br />

was presented near the endogenous saccade goal relative to when it<br />

was presented elsewhere. <strong>The</strong> results indicate that the presentation of<br />

the stop signal exogenously activates the oculomotor system and interacts<br />

with the endogenous saccade program, providing evidence for<br />

competitive integration models of oculomotor control.<br />

(3067)<br />

Emotion Potentiates Response Activation and Inhibition in Masked<br />

Motor Priming. BRUNO R. BOCANEGRA & RENÉ ZEELENBERG,<br />

Erasmus University Rotterdam (sponsored by Ingmar H. Franken)—<br />

Emotional or arousing stimuli are known to enhance the allocation of<br />

visual attention and to improve early perceptual processing. In the<br />

present study, we examined whether emotional stimuli also potentiate<br />

the activation and inhibition of automatic motor responses. We inves-<br />

96<br />

tigated whether peripherally presented fearful faces influence the response<br />

times to foveally presented target stimuli that were preceded by<br />

masked prime stimuli. Primes and targets elicited either left- or righthand<br />

responses, at stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) of 20 or<br />

170 msec. We show that both early facilitative (SOA: 20 msec) as well<br />

as late inhibitory (SOA: 170 msec) RT effects due to prime–target<br />

compatibility are enhanced through the presence of peripheral fearful<br />

faces. In sum, our combined findings suggest that emotion causes both<br />

a response-channel-specific enhancement of motor inhibition and a<br />

response-channel-independent enhancement of motor activation.<br />

• DECISION MAKING •<br />

(3068)<br />

Aggressive Shooting Behavior: <strong>The</strong> Influence of Span, Personality,<br />

and Threat on Shoot Decisions. HEATHER M. KLEIDER &<br />

DOMINIC PARROTT, Georgia State University—We examined whether<br />

individual differences in personality and cognitive processing ability<br />

influenced shoot decisions in a simulated shooting task. Specifically,<br />

we tested, for trait-aggressive people, whether cognitive processing<br />

ability (i.e., working memory) influenced their decision to shoot when<br />

they were feeling threatened. <strong>The</strong> results showed that although high<br />

cognitive control (high-span) people are skilled at suppressing extraneous<br />

information, compared with their low-span counterparts, and<br />

maintained superior performance (accurate shooting of armed suspects)<br />

when in the no-threat condition, their shooting behavior was<br />

markedly more aggressive (shooting unarmed suspects) when threatened.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings suggest that the cognitive processing ability that<br />

trait-aggressive high-span persons rely on to control aggressive behavior<br />

is reduced when they are threatened, resulting in aggressive<br />

shooting behavior. Implications for in-field police officers are<br />

discussed.<br />

(3069)<br />

Regulatory Fit Effects in a Gambling Task. DARRELL A. WORTHY,<br />

W. TODD MADDOX, & ARTHUR B. MARKMAN, University of Texas,<br />

Austin (sponsored by W. Todd Maddox)—We examined the interface<br />

between motivation and choice. In category learning, a regulatory fit<br />

has been shown to increase exploration of alternative response strategies<br />

even when exploration is suboptimal. In the current study, promotion<br />

and prevention focus subjects performed a gambling task that<br />

required them to choose from one of two decks on each trial. <strong>The</strong>y either<br />

gained or lost points with each draw. In Experiment 1, optimal<br />

performance required an exploratory response pattern that entailed<br />

sampling from a deck that initially appeared disadvantageous but ultimately<br />

became advantageous. In Experiment 2, optimal performance<br />

required an exploitative response pattern. A softmax reinforcement<br />

learning model that includes an exploitation parameter was applied to<br />

the data and revealed greater exploration of alternative strategies for<br />

people with a regulatory fit. This response strategy was optimal in Experiment<br />

1 and led to superior performance, but was suboptimal in Experiment<br />

2 and led to inferior performance.<br />

(3070)<br />

Actual Versus Hypothetical Decision Making. KATHLEEN M.<br />

GALOTTI & LAURA R. NATHAN, Carleton College—Parents of<br />

kindergarteners (n = 243) participated in a short-term longitudinal<br />

study, as they made a decision for educational placement (from among<br />

7–8 available options) for their child for the following year. A second<br />

sample of undergraduates (n = 45) were also asked to pretend to make<br />

this decision for their hypothetical child, and the responses were compared.<br />

Analyses will focus on similarities and differences in the responses<br />

of these two groups, one of whom is making an actual decision,<br />

and the other, a simulated one. Measures examined include the number<br />

of options and criteria under active consideration, the number and type<br />

of information sources consulted, the number and type of goals the<br />

decision-maker listed, and the decision-making style the decisionmaker<br />

reported using in making this and other important decisions.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!