29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Papers 55–61 Friday Morning<br />

results suggest that response-signal experiments and Remember–Know<br />

judgments measure different aspects of a recognition process, possibly<br />

providing a coherent explanation for the two contradicting findings.<br />

10:20–10:35 (55)<br />

Perceptual Disfluency Effects in Recognition Memory. DAVID E.<br />

HUBER, University of California, San Diego, TEDRA FAZENDEIRO,<br />

University of Denver, PIOTR WINKIELMAN, University of California,<br />

San Diego, & TIM CURRAN, University of Colorado—A series of 5<br />

forced-choice recognition experiments demonstrated that immediate<br />

repetition priming produces a gradual transition from positive to negative<br />

priming as a function of the prime duration. Specifically, brief<br />

supraliminal primes produced a recognition preference for primed<br />

words (Experiments 1, 2, and 5) whereas long-duration primes produced<br />

a recognition preference against primed words (Experiments<br />

3–5). Experiment 2 demonstrated that priming shifts occur even<br />

when participants accurately recognize brief primes. In Experiments<br />

3–5, a cued recall task revealed that priming effects only occur<br />

for recognition without recall, and in Experiment 4, independent manipulation<br />

of the recollective and familiarity components of recognition<br />

provided further evidence that priming affects familiarity. All experiments<br />

included a variety of controls against strategic discounting,<br />

supporting the claim that negative priming can arise naturally as a result<br />

of the transition from perceptual fluency to perceptual disfluency.<br />

10:40–10:55 (56)<br />

Confidence and Accuracy in Recognition Memory for Faces. AN-<br />

DREW J. HEATHCOTE & MELISSA PRINCE, University of Newcastle—Signal<br />

detection analysis indicates that studied (old) items<br />

have both a greater mean and SD for memory strength than do unstudied<br />

(new) items. Supporting evidence comes from Receiver Operating<br />

Curves (ROCs) based on new–old choice confidence, and so<br />

it assumes that confidence and accuracy depend on the same underlying<br />

memory-strength dimension. In contrast, Koriat’s (1997) accessibility<br />

theory assumes that other factors may influence confidence.<br />

Busey, Tunnicliff, Loftus, and Loftus (2000) examined this issue using<br />

Bamber’s (1979) state-trace analysis, which supported a two-dimensional<br />

model of the confidence–accuracy relationship. <strong>The</strong>se results, and<br />

ROC results for data averaged over participants, were interpreted as<br />

supporting accessibility theory. However, subject average ROC results<br />

can be misleading (Malmberg & Xu, 2006). We replicated Busey et al.<br />

and a related experiment (Loftus, Oberg, & Dylan, 2004) and applied<br />

ROC analysis to individual participant data to further test the nature<br />

of the confidence–accuracy relation.<br />

11:00–11:15 (57)<br />

<strong>The</strong> “Testing Effect” in Recognition Memory. K. A. CHALMERS<br />

& H. E. TURON, University of Newcastle—<strong>The</strong> “testing effect” refers<br />

to the finding that completion of a memory test not only assesses what<br />

one has learned, but also increases memory performance on a subsequent<br />

test. <strong>The</strong> role of word frequency, study duration, and orthographic<br />

distinctiveness on the magnitude of the testing effect was examined<br />

in a series of recognition memory experiments. In each<br />

experiment, undergraduate students studied a list of words prior to<br />

completing two yes/no recognition memory tests. On Test 1, recognition<br />

of studied (targets) and nonstudied items (distractors) was tested.<br />

On Test 2, recognition of items from the Test 1 list, plus new distractor<br />

items was tested. <strong>The</strong> testing effect was observed, with recognition<br />

accuracy higher for Test 2 than for Test 1. <strong>The</strong> magnitude of the effect<br />

varied over manipulations of word frequency, study duration, and orthographic<br />

distinctiveness. Whether the results support explanations<br />

based on retrieval effects or changes in decision criteria is discussed.<br />

11:20–11:35 (58)<br />

Repetition and Memory: A Multitask Approach. DOUGLAS L.<br />

HINTZMAN, University of Oregon—Recently there has been a reemergence<br />

of cumulative strength models of the effects of repetition<br />

on episodic memory. Strength is a scalar construct, and these models<br />

9<br />

are generally tested against a scalar memory measure—typically,<br />

recognition d′. But many memory tasks have been devised, and each<br />

provides a different window onto the operation of the same memory<br />

system. I will describe research using several different memoryjudgment<br />

tasks, employing a method that looks at the degree of independence,<br />

or dissociation, among memory measures. <strong>The</strong> results of<br />

this research pose a serious challenge to the claim that repetition simply<br />

strengthens a memory trace.<br />

11:40–11:55 (59)<br />

A Simplified Conjoint Recognition Paradigm for the Measurement<br />

of Gist and Verbatim Memory. CHRISTOPH STAHL & CHRISTOPH<br />

KLAUER, University of Freiburg (sponsored by Ute J. Bayen)—<strong>The</strong><br />

distinction between verbatim and gist memory traces postulated by<br />

Fuzzy Trace theory has furthered our understanding of numerous phenomena<br />

in various fields, such as false memory research, research on<br />

reasoning and decision making, and cognitive development. To measure<br />

verbatim and gist memory empirically, an experimental paradigm<br />

and multinomial measurement model has been proposed, but rarely<br />

applied. In the present article, a simplified Conjoint Recognition paradigm<br />

and multinomial model is introduced and validated as a measurement<br />

tool for the separate assessment of verbatim and gist memory<br />

processes. A Bayesian metacognitive framework is applied to<br />

validate guessing processes. Similarities and differences of the new<br />

paradigm and the Source Monitoring paradigm are also highlighted.<br />

Language Production<br />

Shoreline, Friday Morning, 10:00–12:00<br />

Chaired by Lise Abrams, University of Florida<br />

10:00–10:15 (60)<br />

Grammatical Class Influences How Nonwords Prime Tip-of-the-<br />

Tongue Resolution. LISE ABRAMS, University of Florida,<br />

KATHERINE K. WHITE, College of Charleston, LISA A. MER-<br />

RILL, University of Florida, & LEE ANNE HAUSLER, College of<br />

Charleston—Previous research shows that phonologically related<br />

words help resolve tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states, but only for words<br />

in a different grammatical class from the TOT word. Two experiments<br />

used phonologically related nonwords to examine whether existing<br />

lexical representations were necessary for priming. Participants saw<br />

questions and attempted to produce target answers. <strong>The</strong>y categorized<br />

unretrieved targets as TOTs or unknown words and heard a list containing<br />

words and nonwords, one of which was a nonword prime or<br />

unrelated nonword. Primes contained the target’s first syllable and had<br />

a suffix consistent with the target’s grammatical class, different from<br />

the target’s grammatical class, or unassociated with grammatical<br />

class. When target retrieval was re-attempted, different part-of-speech<br />

primes facilitated TOT resolution whereas same part-of-speech<br />

primes did not, similar to studies using word primes. Primes without<br />

grammatical suffixes also increased TOT resolution. <strong>The</strong> results<br />

demonstrate that nonwords can activate grammatical class, influencing<br />

subsequent production of phonology for resolving TOTs.<br />

10:20–10:35 (61)<br />

A Word-Order Constraint on Phonological Activation. NIELS<br />

JANSSEN & F.-XAVIER ALARIO, CNRS and Université de Provence<br />

(sponsored by Jonathan Grainger)—In many languages, word-order<br />

rules impose major constraints on linguistic behavior. Despite their<br />

importance, little is known about how these rules operate. We report<br />

an influence of word-order on the activation of phonological representations<br />

during language production. Participants were presented<br />

with colored objects (e.g., blue rake), and named either the color (e.g.,<br />

blue) or the object (e.g., rake). <strong>The</strong> phonological onset similarity between<br />

color and object name was manipulated (e.g., red rake vs. blue<br />

rake). In Experiment 1, French speakers showed a phonological congruency<br />

effect in color but, surprisingly, not in object naming. In Experiment<br />

2, English speakers yielded the opposite pattern: A phono-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!