29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Friday Noon Posters 2104–2111<br />

mance does, however, lead to better sensitivity. Overall, conformance<br />

accounts for 85% of the variance across both locations.<br />

(2104)<br />

Multiple Somatosensory Frames of Reference: Evidence From the<br />

Simon Effect. JARED MEDINA, University of Pennsylvania, &<br />

BRENDA RAPP, Johns Hopkins University—Individuals respond more<br />

slowly even in nonspatial tasks when the stimulus and response are on<br />

different sides of space—this is known as the Simon effect (Simon &<br />

Small, 1969). Explanations assume that spatial codes are automatically<br />

generated for both stimulus and response within specific spatial<br />

reference frames. We present the results of a novel application of the<br />

“Simon effect paradigm” to examine tactile reference frames. We report<br />

evidence of multiple tactile reference frames. Specifically, when<br />

participants’ arms are uncrossed, we find that allocentric, objectrelative<br />

tactile stimulus codes are generated. However, when participants’<br />

arms are crossed, an egocentric, somatotopic reference frame<br />

is engaged (e.g., the left hand is encoded as left regardless of hand position).<br />

We discuss these findings within a tactile processing framework<br />

that includes somatotopic and nonsomatotopic stages of spatial<br />

representation and processing.<br />

• SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS •<br />

(2105)<br />

Selecting a Reference Object. LAURA A. CARLSON, MARK R.<br />

WILLIAMS, & PATRICK L. HILL, University of Notre Dame—Spatial<br />

descriptions such as “<strong>The</strong> stapler is behind the binder” specify the<br />

location of a target (stapler) by spatially relating it to a reference object<br />

(binder) whose location is assumed to be easily found due to its<br />

salience relative to other surrounding objects. Using line drawings and<br />

real object displays, we assessed the importance of three candidate dimensions<br />

for defining salience: spatial, perceptual, and conceptual.<br />

Participants described the location of a target within displays containing<br />

multiple candidate reference objects. For spatial salience, candidate<br />

objects were placed in more preferred or less preferred spatial<br />

relations to the target. For perceptual salience, one candidate reference<br />

object was uniquely colored. For conceptual salience, one candidate<br />

reference object was the criterion object in a prior categorization task.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results indicate a strong preference to select reference objects on<br />

the basis of their spatial relation to the target, with perceptual attributes<br />

having a stronger influence than conceptual attributes.<br />

(2106)<br />

Perspective in Spatial Memory and Descriptions: Transforming<br />

Representations for Communication. SARAH KRIZ, Naval Research<br />

Laboratory, & MARY HEGARTY, University of California, Santa<br />

Barbara—This study evaluated how spatial perspective is mentally<br />

represented and the extent to which limits in representational flexibility<br />

affect linguistic communication. In three experiments, participants<br />

learned an environment through direct navigation or map viewing.<br />

Later, they were asked to describe the environment from a<br />

perspective that matched or mismatched their learned perspective. Descriptions<br />

were analyzed to test three representational models: (1) spatial<br />

representations are perspective-free, (2) spatial representations<br />

encode multiple perspectives, and (3) spatial representations encode<br />

the learned perspective, and are transformed to access the other perspective<br />

when required by the communicative situation. Data from the<br />

three experiments were best explained by the transformation model,<br />

and the results suggest that transformations of spatial representations<br />

to an unlearned perspective are subject to cognitive limitations.<br />

(2107)<br />

Cue Effects on Memory for Location When Navigating Spatial<br />

Displays. SYLVIA FITTING, DOUGLAS H. WEDELL, & GARY L.<br />

ALLEN, University of South Carolina (sponsored by David E.<br />

Clement)—Participants maneuvered a mouse icon smoothly through<br />

a circular region on the computer screen to find a hidden platform in a<br />

84<br />

procedure that blended aspects of the Morris water maze task and the<br />

dot location task. Predictions for effects of varying number of surrounding<br />

cues (1, 2, or 3) were generated from a model of cue-based<br />

memory error and bias (Fitting, Wedell, & Allen, in press). <strong>The</strong> pattern<br />

of biases and errors was consistent with those predictions. Memory performance<br />

decreased with decrease in number of cues, as measured by<br />

number of moves to reach the target location and absolute heading error.<br />

As memory performance declined, bias effects increased, as measured<br />

by heading bias in the first 10 navigational steps and positional bias in<br />

the last 10 steps. Remembered locations were biased toward the nearest<br />

cue, and error decreased as a function of proximity to cue locations.<br />

(2108)<br />

To Go in Circles or Forge Straight Ahead: Depicting Cyclical<br />

Processes. ANGELA KESSELL, Stanford University, & BARBARA<br />

TVERSKY, Columbia University and Stanford University—Schematic<br />

forms in depictions, such as lines, boxes, and arrows, can convey<br />

meanings about concepts and relations that are readily understood in<br />

context. Here, we investigate production and comprehension of depictions<br />

of cyclical processes such as cell division and washing<br />

clothes. When restricted to a circular display, people tend to put a natural<br />

first step in the 12 o’clock position, with subsequent steps clockwise;<br />

they also interpret the 12 o’clock step as the first one unless<br />

there is a natural first step elsewhere. When not restricted to a circular<br />

display, people tend to depict cyclical processes linearly.<br />

(2109)<br />

Learning the Layout of a New Building: A Longitudinal, Real-World<br />

Study. DREW DARA-ABRAMS & MARY HEGARTY, University of<br />

California, Santa Barbara (sponsored by Jack M. Loomis)—Over the<br />

course of an academic year, we followed faculty members and graduate<br />

students as they learned the layout of a new addition to the psychology<br />

building at the University of California, Santa Barbara. At<br />

three times during the year, participants generated route directions between<br />

locations in the new and old buildings, which are connected by<br />

catwalks spanning a courtyard. As each of the three floors in both<br />

buildings has a different layout, route directions appeared to follow<br />

one of three strategies: first head in the destination’s direction, first<br />

head to the destination’s floor, or first go to the ground floor. Route<br />

efficiency was evaluated based on path length, and floor plans were<br />

used to compute a visibility graph, which indicated which routes were<br />

the most visually integrated and contained the fewest turns. We will<br />

report the effects of both visibility and learning on strategy choice.<br />

(2110)<br />

Inducing Hierarchical Representations: Consequences for Spatial<br />

Heuristics. SIMON J. BUECHNER & CHRISTOPH HOELSCHER,<br />

University of Freiburg (sponsored by Daniel R. Montello)—Two experiments<br />

investigated people’s mental representation of a regularly<br />

shaped building and related path choice heuristics. Experiment 1 applied<br />

a structure mapping task, indicating that people spontaneously<br />

group landmarks within their mental representation according to<br />

structural characteristics of the environment (horizontally/vertically).<br />

We also showed that the selection of a path choice heuristic was directly<br />

related to the representation’s structure. Experiment 2 induced<br />

one of the two major groupings in the mental representation by leading<br />

the participants through the building either horizontally or vertically.<br />

Participants performed verification and production tasks with<br />

landmarks along an imagined path. Reaction times in within-group<br />

tasks were faster than those in across-group tasks, showing that participants<br />

organized their representation according to the way they<br />

were exposed to the building. <strong>The</strong> experiments provide evidence that<br />

a person’s mental representation is sensitive to both the environment’s<br />

structure as well as to the way it is experienced.<br />

(2111)<br />

Learning of Absolute and Relative Distance and Direction by<br />

Pigeons. BRADLEY R. STURZ, Armstrong Atlantic State University,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!