29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 2069–2075 Friday Noon<br />

trieval of the Indonesian–English pair and enhanced retrieval of only<br />

the keyword subtask. However, higher imagery value keywords influenced<br />

both the keyword and English retrieval subtasks while making<br />

little or no difference in overall vocabulary retrieval performance.<br />

This unexpected but interesting pattern of results is discussed and potentially<br />

explained.<br />

(2069)<br />

Constrained Retrieval in Recognition Memory? JUSTIN KANTNER<br />

& D. STEPHEN LINDSAY, University of Victoria (sponsored by<br />

D. Stephen Lindsay)—Some prominent theories of recognition memory<br />

hold that recognition judgments are based on the degree of match<br />

between test probes and all previously encountered exemplars. Jacoby<br />

and his colleagues, by contrast, have recently argued that recognition<br />

judgments may be supported in part by a process of “constrained retrieval”<br />

whereby individuals selectively search memory for items from<br />

a particular contextual source. We critique Jacoby’s evidence for this<br />

claim and report new experiments designed to detect constrained retrieval<br />

in a recognition task. <strong>The</strong> results indicate that subjects can indeed<br />

constrain retrieval in response to test probes under some conditions,<br />

but not under others. We discuss the implications of this pattern<br />

of significant and null effects for theories of recognition memory.<br />

(2070)<br />

Pattern Classification of fMRI Retrieval States During Recognition<br />

Memory. JOEL R. QUAMME & KENNETH A. NORMAN, Princeton<br />

University—Pattern classification algorithms have been used in fMRI<br />

to examine distributed activity patterns in the brain associated with<br />

different cognitive states. In this study, subjects studied singular and<br />

plural words, and performed recognition memory tasks in the scanner.<br />

We trained a simple neural network classifier to distinguish between<br />

subjects’ fMRI brain patterns when paying attention to recollected details<br />

about a previously studied item, and when paying attention to<br />

general feelings of item familiarity. <strong>The</strong>n, in a second phase, subjects<br />

made old–new judgments to studied, nonstudied, and switched-plural<br />

versions of studied words (e.g., study fleas, test flea). After training<br />

the classifier on the first phase, we tested it on brain activity from the<br />

second phase to obtain the classifier’s “best guess” concerning the<br />

subject’s retrieval state during old, new, and switched-plural trials. We<br />

report on the relationship between the classifier’s output and subjects’<br />

behavioral responses in this task.<br />

(2071)<br />

Neuroimaging and Electromyographic Evidence for Response<br />

Conflict in Recognition Tasks. TRAVIS L. SEYMOUR, University<br />

of California, Santa Cruz, & ERIC H. SCHUMACHER, Georgia Institute<br />

of Technology—How do memory retrieval processes lead to<br />

overt responses in strategic recognition tasks? <strong>The</strong> Jacoby model proposes<br />

that the influences of both familiarity and recollection processes<br />

combine to select appropriate recognition responses, but does not account<br />

for response times (RTs). An extended model designed to account<br />

for accuracy and RT (Seymour, 2001) suggests that parallel familiarity<br />

and recollection-based task-sets can lead to response conflict<br />

that must be resolved by control processes. We tested this prediction<br />

by using brain activation and electromyographic measures during an<br />

exclude recognition task. In Experiment 1, we measured brain activation<br />

from cortical regions sensitive to response conflict and found<br />

greater activity for conflict than for filler trials. Experiment 2 used<br />

electromyographic evidence to replicate this finding with a measure<br />

incontrovertibly related to motor execution. <strong>The</strong>se results are consistent<br />

with the parallel task-set model’s assumption that recognition,<br />

motor, and conflict monitoring processes interact in strategic retrieval<br />

tasks.<br />

(2072)<br />

Retrieval Process of Autobiographical Memory and Oldness of<br />

Episode. TAKASHI HORIUCHI, Okayama University—<strong>The</strong> purpose<br />

of this study was to examine how the difference of oldness of episode<br />

79<br />

affected the retrieval process of an autobiographical memory. Sixteen<br />

university students participated in the experiment. In each trial, the<br />

participant was assigned to either the university or the junior high<br />

school period condition. In both conditions, the participant was asked<br />

to retrieve an episode associated with each cue word, and reported<br />

“remember,” “know,” or “no.” Estimates derived from an independent<br />

remember–know procedure (Jacoby, 1998) showed that the university<br />

period condition was higher in both the intentional and the automatic<br />

process than was the junior high school period condition.<br />

• METACOGNITION •<br />

(2073)<br />

Exploring Undetected Errors in Skilled Cognitive Activity.<br />

LESLIE A. ADERHOLD & RICHARD A. CARLSON, Pennsylvania<br />

State University—Increased skill typically leads to faster performance<br />

and fewer errors. <strong>The</strong>se improvements result in part from more concise<br />

mental representations. However, more concise representations<br />

may sometimes contribute to characteristic errors and failures of monitoring<br />

at high levels of skill. For example, even skilled writers may<br />

find it difficult to proofread their own work. Participants in several<br />

studies practiced a rule-application task, checking for rule violations<br />

and reporting confidence in their responses. Some error types persisted<br />

at high levels of skill, even when speed–accuracy trade-offs<br />

were ruled out. Furthermore, participants often reported very high<br />

confidence in error responses, persisted in those errors, and continued<br />

to report high confidence when provided the opportunity to recheck<br />

their work. We consider possible explanations in terms of motivation,<br />

implicit monitoring on the basis of fluency, and representational constraints<br />

on metacognition. Understanding failures of error monitoring<br />

in high-performance skills may have important practical implications.<br />

(2074)<br />

Metacomprehension of Statistical Concepts. RUTH H. MAKI,<br />

CURTIS CRAIG, J. L. RUDINE, & ELIZABETH A. MURRAY, Texas<br />

Tech University—We investigated transfer appropriate monitoring<br />

(TAM) in metacomprehension of statistical concepts. During learning,<br />

participants either calculated answers to problems or read the answers.<br />

Learning was tested with multiple-choice and calculation tests.<br />

TAM suggests that calculating during learning would lead to more accurate<br />

judgments on the calculation test, and reading during learning<br />

would lead to more accurate judgments on the multiple-choice tests.<br />

<strong>The</strong> learning activity � test interaction was not significant with metacomprehension<br />

accuracy, but it did occur with bias. Calculating during<br />

learning produced overconfidence on the multiple-choice test, but<br />

it had no effect on the calculation test. Overall, relative prediction accuracy<br />

was higher for participants who had previously taken a statistics<br />

course and for participants who did not calculate during learning.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest that calculation during learning interferes with<br />

students’ abilities to judge how well they are learning, and it may lead<br />

to overconfidence.<br />

(2075)<br />

Study Strategies—Do Deaf College Students Do It Differently?<br />

MARY DIANE CLARK & LASHAUN A. WILLIAMS, Gallaudet<br />

University—Looking at the literature on test-taking strategies, underachieving<br />

groups tended to show less preparation when studying for<br />

tests and developed fewer strategies for the behavior, and those strategies<br />

tended to be ineffective (Bornhold, 2002; Ellis & Ryan, 2003).<br />

In addition, Ellis and Ryan found that these students had lower levels<br />

of test-taking self-efficacy. More specifically for deaf test-takers,<br />

Chaliff and Toranzo (2000) found that the test construction also impacted<br />

their students’ results on these kinds of tests. <strong>The</strong> English syntax<br />

of the questions as well as the question format caused deaf testtakers<br />

difficulty in demonstrating their knowledge. Chaliff and<br />

Toranzo related these difficulties to poor reading strategies. <strong>The</strong> current<br />

study elicited strategies from a group of deaf college students.<br />

Content analysis shows more superficial strategies that appear to re-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!