29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Friday Noon Posters 2062–2068<br />

ness. In addition, we report that judgments are influenced by working<br />

memory, the number of sequences judged per set, as well as whether<br />

the data are presented as a series of discrete outcomes or in a graphical<br />

format. We conclude that when judging sequences, people are not<br />

as biased as has been previously reported.<br />

(2062)<br />

Object Familiarity Can Be Altered in the Presence of Other Objects.<br />

ALAN S. BROWN, Southern Methodist University, & ELIZABETH J.<br />

MARSH, Duke University—Does the familiarity of an object depend<br />

on the context in which it is judged? To answer this, abstract symbols<br />

were judged alone or in the context of another symbol. <strong>The</strong> flanker<br />

symbol was either very familiar or very unfamiliar, as established in<br />

a pilot study. Experiment 1 used two brief presentation conditions:<br />

100 or 1,000 msec. In both conditions, unfamiliar flankers decreased<br />

rated target familiarity, while familiar flankers increased rated target<br />

familiarity, relative to the no-flanker control. In Experiment 2, symbols<br />

remained in view until rated. Familiar flankers continued to inflate<br />

the familiarity of the target, but there was no effect of unfamiliar<br />

flankers (relative to the no-flanker control). Irrelevant stimuli can<br />

change how familiar a target feels, especially when the nontarget is<br />

very familiar.<br />

(2063)<br />

Implicit Figural–Semantic Association in Chinese Characters.<br />

MIN-JU LIAO, HAN-JU TASI, & CHING-FAN SHEU, National Cheng<br />

Kung University—Research has shown a figural–semantic association<br />

in Chinese where the orthographic signs are used to designate concepts.<br />

<strong>The</strong> present study investigated an implicit/automatic process of<br />

this association. Our hypothesis was that figural attributes of positive<br />

meaning characters will automatically prompt positive perceptual<br />

evaluations. Perceptual judgment and recognition tasks were employed<br />

in the present experiment. In the perceptual judgment task, participants<br />

noncognate of Chinese were presented with a pair of Chinese<br />

characters designed with either antonymic or neutral concepts and<br />

were to choose one character that was perceptually more preferable.<br />

In the recognition task, participants’ recognition performance for<br />

characters presented in the first task was examined. Preliminary data<br />

revealed that when presented with two antonymic characters, participants<br />

tended to evaluate positive meaning characters as more preferable.<br />

<strong>The</strong> recognition rates for individual characters from antonymic<br />

or neutral concept pairs did not show any difference.<br />

(2064)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of Forecasts From Sources Differing in Forecast Extremity<br />

and Diagnosticity. YARON SHLOMI & THOMAS S. WALLSTEN,<br />

University of Maryland—An important issue concerns how consumers<br />

of forecasts learn to utilize the information provided to them.<br />

We created advisors that varied in forecast extremity (e.g., probability<br />

forecasts that were low [60%] vs. high [90%]) and diagnosticity<br />

(e.g., conditional hit rate, .60 vs. .90) and provided participants an opportunity<br />

to learn about the forecasts’ characteristics over many trials.<br />

<strong>The</strong> data suggest that the two dimensions have independent effects<br />

on the extremity of the participants’ judgments. We developed a mathematical<br />

model to account for participants’ learning in this task. Applying<br />

the model at the level of individual respondents, the estimated<br />

parameters suggest that forecast extremity was overweighted and forecast<br />

diagnosticity was underweighted. Our results are useful on both<br />

theoretical and practical grounds. In addition, the experimental procedure<br />

developed here may be adapted for investigating effects of properties<br />

related to the producer of the forecasts, such as trustworthiness.<br />

• RETRIEVAL PROCESSES •<br />

(20<strong>65</strong>)<br />

Detecting Semantic Versus Episodic Memory Retrieval via fMRI<br />

Pattern Classification. PER B. SEDERBERG & KENNETH A.<br />

NORMAN, Princeton University—Behavioral studies have demon-<br />

78<br />

strated that humans rely on both semantic and episodic associations<br />

to cue memory retrieval; however, with few exceptions, imaging studies<br />

of memory retrieval have focused on the neural correlates of semantic<br />

and episodic memory independently. In order to understand<br />

the interplay between semantic and episodic associations during<br />

memory retrieval, it is first necessary to track subjects’ retrieval states<br />

during memory search. To this end, we collected fMRI data while<br />

subjects performed both semantic and episodic paired-associate stem<br />

completion tasks. We were able to distinguish between semantic and<br />

episodic cue states in all subjects by means of a back-propagation<br />

neural-network pattern classifier. In a subset of the subjects, the output<br />

of the neural classifier also correlated with behavioral performance<br />

in the tasks. <strong>The</strong>se results indicate that subjects modulate their<br />

retrieval state during memory search and that pattern classification<br />

techniques can dissociate between episodic and semantic states during<br />

memory retrieval.<br />

(2066)<br />

Memory Retrieval As an Encoding Event: <strong>The</strong> Role of Attention.<br />

NICOLE M. DUDUKOVIC & ANTHONY D. WAGNER, Stanford<br />

University—When you remember an event, the act of memory retrieval<br />

serves to reencode the experience, ultimately influencing<br />

whether you will remember it in the future. We investigated whether<br />

dividing attention during retrieval reduces the power of retrieval as an<br />

encoding event. Participants studied object pictures under two different<br />

semantic encoding tasks. <strong>The</strong>y then engaged in a recognition and<br />

source memory test, sometimes while simultaneously performing an<br />

auditory discrimination task. Two days later they returned for a second<br />

recognition and source memory test. Recognition and source<br />

memory were better for items that had been retrieved under full as opposed<br />

to divided attention. More importantly, items that were correctly<br />

recognized on the first test were more likely to be subsequently<br />

recognized on the second test if they had initially been retrieved under<br />

full attention. <strong>The</strong> same held true for correct source memories. Thus,<br />

attention may be critical for retrieval benefits.<br />

(2067)<br />

Distributed Retrieval Practice: How Much Is Enough? MARY A.<br />

PYC & KATHERINE A. RAWSON, Kent State University—Previous<br />

research has established that distributed retrieval practice promotes<br />

memory. However, students necessarily have a finite amount of time<br />

to spend studying, and thus examining the efficiency of practice<br />

schedules is also important. In two experiments, we compared various<br />

schedules of distributed retrieval practice to examine the minimum<br />

number of times an item needs to be correctly recalled during<br />

practice to maximize final test performance. <strong>The</strong> results showed that<br />

correctly recalling an item only once during practice led to lower final<br />

test performance than correctly recalling an item more than once during<br />

practice. However, we observed diminishing returns of further<br />

practice, until a point was reached where correctly recalling an item<br />

more times during practice provided no further benefit to final test<br />

performance. <strong>The</strong> results also showed that the level of practice at<br />

which asymptotic final test performance was reached depended on the<br />

retention interval between practice and final test.<br />

(2068)<br />

Phonological and Visual Imagery Mediation Processes in Vocabulary<br />

Retrieval. ROBERT J. CRUTCHER & JENNIFER M. SHEA,<br />

University of Dayton—Two experiments investigated retrieval processes<br />

of vocabulary pairs learned using keyword mediators by independently<br />

manipulating the phonological or visual imagery properties of<br />

the mediators. We measured accuracy and latency of retrieving<br />

Indonesian–English vocabulary pairs (vocabulary task), as well as of<br />

retrieving keywords from Indonesian words (keyword subtask) and<br />

English translations from keywords (English subtask). Manipulating<br />

the phonological similarity of the keyword mediator to the Indonesian<br />

word impacted vocabulary retrieval such that greater phonological<br />

similarity of keyword mediators to Indonesian words enhanced re-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!