S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Friday Noon Posters 2048–2054 ing training, the comparison response alternatives were always presented following remember (R) cues, but never presented following forget (F) cues. During testing, accuracy was significantly poorer on F-cued trials than on R-cued trials. A choose-short bias was observed at extended delays on both R-cued and F-cued trials. However, increasing the duration of the R cues and F cues produced a choose-long bias, indicating that rats responded to comparisons on the basis of the combined duration of the sample and the postsample cue. This is the first demonstration of a directed forgetting effect for event duration memory in rats. (2048) Can Big Brown Bats Integrate Their Visual and Echoic Sensory Systems? CAROLINE M. DELONG & JAMES A. SIMMONS, Brown University—Big brown bats echolocate by emitting wideband frequencymodulated sonar sounds and processing the echoes that return to their ears. Little is known about their ability to integrate their echolocation system with their visual system during object discrimination tasks. In this experiment, bats were presented with a two-alternative forced choice task in which the stimuli were a one-cylinder monopole target (negative stimulus) and a two-cylinder dipole target (positive stimulus). In the training phase, the stimuli were painted white and the task was done in dim light so that the bats could use both echolocation and vision. In the testing phase, the stimuli were presented in two unimodal conditions (echolocation only and vision only) and one bimodal condition in which one cylinder of the dipole target was presented visually and the other echoically. If the bats succeed in the bimodal condition, it implies that they can integrate these two sensory systems. (2049) Ultrasonic Vocalizations With Dorsal Stimulation in Rat Pups. CHRISTOPHER WILSON, JEFFREY S. ANASTASI, KRISTINA NUNGARAY, YU KUBOTA, & MANUELA GARZA, Sam Houston State University—Three experiments were performed to investigate production of ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) in rat pups in response to transport response elicitation with dorsal stimulation, using both quantitative and qualitative measures. In Experiment 1, USVs were recorded with tonic (holding) or phasic (gently waving) stimulation with phasic producing increases in USV rate. In Experiment 2, USVs were recorded with the pup passively held, suspended to induce the dorsal immobility response, and suspended so as to induce a transport response. Transport response elicitation increased number of USVs above both dorsal immobility and passive handling. In Experiment 3, USVs were recorded just prior to the mother’s retrieving the pup and during retrieval. Actual retrieval increased both number and duration of USVs, producing both quantitative and qualitative differences, over preretrieval. The results are discussed with respect to the nature of the pups’ responses, input of maternal stimulation, and possible mechanisms for these differences. (2050) Refining the Visual Cortical Priming Hypothesis in Category Learning by Humans and Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta). MARIANA V. C. COUTINHO, JUSTIN J. COUCHMAN, JOSHUA S. REDFORD, & J. DAVID SMITH, University at Buffalo (sponsored by J. David Smith)—Humans show prototype-enhancement effects and steep typicality gradients in dot-distortion categorization tasks. These effects have been ascribed to visual cortical priming. To challenge and refine this hypothesis, we compared performance by humans and monkeys on two dot-distortion category tasks in which (1) the stimuli were size invariant and (2) the stimuli varied greatly in size. Prototype effects and typicality gradients were substantially the same in the two tasks, for humans and monkeys. This constrains the visual cortical priming hypothesis, because low-level visual-priming effects would not show this robustness of performance to size change. There may be higher level priming mechanisms involved in dot-distortion categorization, or prototype-abstraction processes may not actually be solely priming based. 76 (2051) Comparing Implicit and Explicit Categorization Strategies in Humans and Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta). JUSTIN J. COUCHMAN, MARIANA V. C. COUTINHO, & J. DAVID SMITH, University at Buffalo—In an early dissociation between implicit and explicit categorization, Kemler Nelson (1984) gave participants an optional categorization task that could be performed by responding either to overall family resemblance or to a single-dimensional criterial rule. Explicit human learners often adopted the rule-based strategy; incidental human learners often adopted the family-resemblance strategy. In the present study, we replicated Kemler Nelson’s human experiment and found the same dissociation. In addition, we extended her paradigm to rhesus monkeys, asking whether they also have an explicit, category-learning system that can be responsive to analytic rules. Macaques heavily favored the family-resemblance strategy. Even after many sessions and thousands of trials in the task, they seemed to have little appreciation that a simple rule, based on one stimulus dimension, could produce perfect categorization. (2052) Transfer Testing Provides Little Evidence of Commonality Between Matching-to-Duration Tasks Involving Keylight and Food Presentations. DOUGLAS S. GRANT, University of Alberta—Pigeons were trained to discriminate between 2- and 8-sec durations of keylight and food presentations. Different comparison stimuli were presented following keylight and food durations (colors for one task and lines for the other task). Following acquisition, the birds experienced two successive transfer tasks in which the comparisons trained with one set of durations were presented on trials involving the alternate set of durations. In each transfer test, the duration-to-comparison mapping was either consistent (i.e., both short samples associated with one comparison and both long samples associated with the other comparison) or inconsistent (i.e., one short and one long sample associated with one comparison, and the other short and long sample associated with the other comparison). Whether transfer involved a consistent or inconsistent sample-to-comparison mapping had little effect on either initial accuracy or rate of acquisition. Hence, duration tasks using keylight and food durations are not functionally equivalent. (2053) Timing With Opportunity Costs: Concurrent Schedules Improve Accuracy of Peak Timing. FEDERICO SANABRIA, Arizona State University, ERIC A. THRAILKILL, Utah State University, & PETER R. KILLEEN, Arizona State University—The temporal generalization gradient produced by the peak procedure conflates detectability and bias. We instituted concurrent ratio and interval schedules to manipulate the opportunity cost of false alarms (early and late peak responses). Concurrent tasks brought peak start and stop times closer to the target time. The shifts were greatest for start times, which not only moved closer to the target time, but whose coefficients of variation decreased. The changes were inconsistent with several basic models of timing. • DECISION MAKING • (2054) Melioration Versus Maximization: Feedback- and Strategy-Driven Models of Adaptive Behavior. HANSJOERG NETH, CHRIS R. SIMS, & WAYNE D. GRAY, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute (sponsored by Wayne D. Gray)—Environments that confront cognitive agents with a choice between local and global gains often evoke a behavioral pattern known as melioration—a preference for immediate rewards over higher long-term gains (Herrnstein, 1997; Tunney & Shanks, 2002). We previously presented evidence that global feedback about expected and optimal gains does not enable decision makers to reliably overcome melioration in favor of maximization in a probabilistic binary-choice paradigm (Neth, Sims, & Gray, 2006). Treating the dilemma between melioration and maximization as a representational

Posters 2055–2061 Friday Noon problem, we present a range of reinforcement learning models that explore how slight shifts in attended dimensions and reinforced units of behavior can result in different allocation strategies. Beyond mimicking aggregate human data, our models explain how sequential decision making can be understood as a combination of feedback- and strategy-driven processes, effectively reconceptualizing probability matching as representational learning. (2055) Exploring the Supraliminal Mere Exposure Effect. MARC G. BERMAN, RICHARD GONZALEZ, & MICHAEL S. FRANKLIN, University of Michigan—The mere exposure effect is a phenomenon where participants prefer stimuli that have been repeatedly presented as opposed to stimuli that have not been presented. The stimuli in most mere exposure studies are neutral and novel to the participant, therefore eliminating preexisting valence. Here, we explored how the supraliminal mere exposure effect was affected by differently valenced stimuli (as determined by independent raters). In addition, we explored potential factors accounting for the observed heterogeneity whereby some participants show the mere exposure effect and others do not. Furthermore, we present a statistical method for analyzing mere exposure data using ordinal regression that is more sensitive than previously utilized techniques and permits modeling the heterogeneity. Preliminary findings suggest that subjects who do show the effect are not influenced by stimulus valence and utilize recognition to determine stimulus preference. Therefore, it appears that recognition overrides initial stimulus valence in supraliminal mere exposure preference judgments. (2056) A Comparison of Reaction-Time Models of Multiple-Choice Decisions. FABIO LEITE & ROGER RATCLIFF, Ohio State University (sponsored by Roger Ratcliff)—Several sequential sampling models for multiple-choice decisions were evaluated. These models used various racing diffusion processes (e.g., Usher & McClelland’s, 2001, leaky competing accumulator model). The structures of the models differed on a number of dimensions, including starting point of competing accumulators, lateral inhibition, and constraints on accumulation rates (e.g., accumulators only allowed to take nonnegative values). Data were collected from a letter-discrimination experiment in which stimulus difficulty or probability of the response alternatives was varied along with number of response alternatives. Performance of the models was addressed by testing them against empirical data, including issues of psychological plausibility of parameter estimates and mimicry among models. (2057) Effects of Caffeine on Deck Selection in Two Gambling Tasks. PATRICK A. RAMIREZ & DANIEL S. LEVINE, University of Texas, Arlington—Participants were given two different gambling tasks: the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) due to Bechara et al., and a variant of the modified IGT due to Peters and Slovic, in either order, with 100 trials in each. On the second task, the participants were given either a caffeinated drink (treatment), a decaffeinated drink (placebo), or water (control). It was found that caffeine significantly increased the tendency to choose advantageous decks, but only on the Bechara task and not on the Peters task. The advantage of caffeine on the Bechara task was manifest even on the first 20 deck selections. This led us to conjecture that the effect of caffeine could be primarily an enhancement of attention, with the Bechara task being the more subject to inattention because the Peters task has more variability in its outcomes. (2058) Human and Optimal Valuation in a Sequential Decision-Making Task With Uncertainty. KYLER M. EASTMAN, BRIAN J. STANKIE- WICZ, & ALEX C. HUK, University of Texas, Austin—Many sequential sampling models suggest that decisions rely on the accumulation of evidence over time until reaching a particular threshold. 77 These models can often account for variations of speed and accuracy in perceptual tasks by manipulation of this threshold. But how does this threshold get determined in real-world decisions? It has been hypothesized that the threshold maximizes some reward function, possibly incorporating measurements of both speed and accuracy (Gold & Shadlen, 2003). This approach has produced a family of models that accurately describes behavior for two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) tasks. (Bogacz et al., 2006) However, it has been unclear what the optimal threshold becomes when additional perceptual information can be obtained at a cost. We present a model of optimal sequential decision making in a task that extends the traditional 2AFC by adding the option of additional information. In the task, the observer receives a sample from two overlapping distributions. • JUDGMENT • (2059) Associations Between Absent Events in Contingency Judgment. LEYRE CASTRO, FABIÁN A. SOTO, & EDWARD A. WASSERMAN, University of Iowa—Studies of contingency judgment have shown that cue–outcome associations can be strengthened or weakened even when the cues are absent (e.g., retrospective revaluation effects). But, there is no evidence in the mainstream contingency judgment literature for the formation of new associations between an absent cue and an absent outcome. Interestingly, research with human infants (Cuevas, Rovee-Collier, & Learmonth, 2006) and nonhuman animals (Dwyer, Mackintosh, & Boakes, 1998) has disclosed that new associations between cues and outcomes can be formed when neither of them is presented. In our own contingency judgment experiments, we have found evidence for the formation of new associations between cues and outcomes that are absent, but that are associatively activated. Our results raise difficult problems for cognitive or inferential accounts (e.g., De Houwer, Beckers, & Vandorpe, 2005), but they can be readily explained by associative learning models (e.g., Dickinson & Burke, 1996; Van Hamme & Wasserman, 1994). (2060) The Effect of Race on Guilty Verdicts: Order Matters. LESLIE N. KNUYCKY, HEATHER M. KLEIDER, & ASHLEY MYERS, Georgia State University (sponsored by Stephen D. Goldinger)—Factors other than case evidence (i.e., available cognitive capacity, judgment instructions) influence juror verdicts (Goldinger et al., 2003). Racial bias, one factor shown to affect jury judgments (Jones & Kaplan, 2003), is posited as an automatic response often difficult to intentionally control. We examined whether attempts to inhibit bias may “backfire” when race is made especially salient. In two experiments, case presentation order (varying position of black or white defendants) and instructions to ignore race impacted jury decisions. The results showed that with no instruction, black defendants received harsher judgments when presenting white- before black-defendant cases. This order effect was negated in Experiment 2 with instruction to disregard race, as guilty verdicts increased overall for black defendants. This suggests that when race is “noticeable” via presentation order or instruction, racially biased decisions result. Such decisions lead to more severe verdicts for black defendants than when race goes “undetected.” (2061) Judgments of Randomness: Is There a Bias Toward High Alternation? FIONA E. J. MCDONALD & BEN R. NEWELL, University of New South Wales (sponsored by Ben R. Newell)—Extensive research has documented that people are poor generators of random sequences. A key feature of human generated sequences is a tendency for excessive alternation of outcomes. The research on judgments of randomness is not as extensive but it has been found that when judging sequences, there is also a tendency to judge those with excessive alternation as most random. We challenge this notion, finding through a series of experiments only minimal bias in judgments of random-

Friday Noon Posters 2048–2054<br />

ing training, the comparison response alternatives were always presented<br />

following remember (R) cues, but never presented following<br />

forget (F) cues. During testing, accuracy was significantly poorer on<br />

F-cued trials than on R-cued trials. A choose-short bias was observed<br />

at extended delays on both R-cued and F-cued trials. However, increasing<br />

the duration of the R cues and F cues produced a choose-long<br />

bias, indicating that rats responded to comparisons on the basis of the<br />

combined duration of the sample and the postsample cue. This is the<br />

first demonstration of a directed forgetting effect for event duration<br />

memory in rats.<br />

(2048)<br />

Can Big Brown Bats Integrate <strong>The</strong>ir Visual and Echoic Sensory<br />

Systems? CAROLINE M. DELONG & JAMES A. SIMMONS, Brown<br />

University—Big brown bats echolocate by emitting wideband frequencymodulated<br />

sonar sounds and processing the echoes that return to their<br />

ears. Little is known about their ability to integrate their echolocation<br />

system with their visual system during object discrimination tasks. In<br />

this experiment, bats were presented with a two-alternative forced<br />

choice task in which the stimuli were a one-cylinder monopole target<br />

(negative stimulus) and a two-cylinder dipole target (positive stimulus).<br />

In the training phase, the stimuli were painted white and the task<br />

was done in dim light so that the bats could use both echolocation and<br />

vision. In the testing phase, the stimuli were presented in two unimodal<br />

conditions (echolocation only and vision only) and one bimodal condition<br />

in which one cylinder of the dipole target was presented visually<br />

and the other echoically. If the bats succeed in the bimodal condition,<br />

it implies that they can integrate these two sensory systems.<br />

(2049)<br />

Ultrasonic Vocalizations With Dorsal Stimulation in Rat Pups.<br />

CHRISTOPHER WILSON, JEFFREY S. ANASTASI, KRISTINA<br />

NUNGARAY, YU KUBOTA, & MANUELA GARZA, Sam Houston<br />

State University—Three experiments were performed to investigate<br />

production of ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) in rat pups in response<br />

to transport response elicitation with dorsal stimulation, using both<br />

quantitative and qualitative measures. In Experiment 1, USVs were<br />

recorded with tonic (holding) or phasic (gently waving) stimulation<br />

with phasic producing increases in USV rate. In Experiment 2, USVs<br />

were recorded with the pup passively held, suspended to induce the<br />

dorsal immobility response, and suspended so as to induce a transport<br />

response. Transport response elicitation increased number of USVs<br />

above both dorsal immobility and passive handling. In Experiment 3,<br />

USVs were recorded just prior to the mother’s retrieving the pup and<br />

during retrieval. Actual retrieval increased both number and duration<br />

of USVs, producing both quantitative and qualitative differences, over<br />

preretrieval. <strong>The</strong> results are discussed with respect to the nature of the<br />

pups’ responses, input of maternal stimulation, and possible mechanisms<br />

for these differences.<br />

(2050)<br />

Refining the Visual Cortical Priming Hypothesis in Category<br />

Learning by Humans and Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta).<br />

MARIANA V. C. COUTINHO, JUSTIN J. COUCHMAN, JOSHUA S.<br />

REDFORD, & J. DAVID SMITH, University at Buffalo (sponsored by<br />

J. David Smith)—Humans show prototype-enhancement effects and<br />

steep typicality gradients in dot-distortion categorization tasks. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

effects have been ascribed to visual cortical priming. To challenge and<br />

refine this hypothesis, we compared performance by humans and<br />

monkeys on two dot-distortion category tasks in which (1) the stimuli<br />

were size invariant and (2) the stimuli varied greatly in size. Prototype<br />

effects and typicality gradients were substantially the same in<br />

the two tasks, for humans and monkeys. This constrains the visual cortical<br />

priming hypothesis, because low-level visual-priming effects<br />

would not show this robustness of performance to size change. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

may be higher level priming mechanisms involved in dot-distortion<br />

categorization, or prototype-abstraction processes may not actually be<br />

solely priming based.<br />

76<br />

(2051)<br />

Comparing Implicit and Explicit Categorization Strategies in Humans<br />

and Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta). JUSTIN J.<br />

COUCHMAN, MARIANA V. C. COUTINHO, & J. DAVID SMITH,<br />

University at Buffalo—In an early dissociation between implicit and<br />

explicit categorization, Kemler Nelson (1984) gave participants an<br />

optional categorization task that could be performed by responding either<br />

to overall family resemblance or to a single-dimensional criterial<br />

rule. Explicit human learners often adopted the rule-based strategy;<br />

incidental human learners often adopted the family-resemblance strategy.<br />

In the present study, we replicated Kemler Nelson’s human experiment<br />

and found the same dissociation. In addition, we extended<br />

her paradigm to rhesus monkeys, asking whether they also have an explicit,<br />

category-learning system that can be responsive to analytic<br />

rules. Macaques heavily favored the family-resemblance strategy.<br />

Even after many sessions and thousands of trials in the task, they<br />

seemed to have little appreciation that a simple rule, based on one<br />

stimulus dimension, could produce perfect categorization.<br />

(2052)<br />

Transfer Testing Provides Little Evidence of Commonality Between<br />

Matching-to-Duration Tasks Involving Keylight and Food Presentations.<br />

DOUGLAS S. GRANT, University of Alberta—Pigeons were<br />

trained to discriminate between 2- and 8-sec durations of keylight and<br />

food presentations. Different comparison stimuli were presented following<br />

keylight and food durations (colors for one task and lines for<br />

the other task). Following acquisition, the birds experienced two successive<br />

transfer tasks in which the comparisons trained with one set<br />

of durations were presented on trials involving the alternate set of durations.<br />

In each transfer test, the duration-to-comparison mapping was<br />

either consistent (i.e., both short samples associated with one comparison<br />

and both long samples associated with the other comparison)<br />

or inconsistent (i.e., one short and one long sample associated with<br />

one comparison, and the other short and long sample associated with<br />

the other comparison). Whether transfer involved a consistent or inconsistent<br />

sample-to-comparison mapping had little effect on either<br />

initial accuracy or rate of acquisition. Hence, duration tasks using keylight<br />

and food durations are not functionally equivalent.<br />

(2053)<br />

Timing With Opportunity Costs: Concurrent Schedules Improve<br />

Accuracy of Peak Timing. FEDERICO SANABRIA, Arizona State<br />

University, ERIC A. THRAILKILL, Utah State University, & PETER R.<br />

KILLEEN, Arizona State University—<strong>The</strong> temporal generalization<br />

gradient produced by the peak procedure conflates detectability and<br />

bias. We instituted concurrent ratio and interval schedules to manipulate<br />

the opportunity cost of false alarms (early and late peak responses).<br />

Concurrent tasks brought peak start and stop times closer to<br />

the target time. <strong>The</strong> shifts were greatest for start times, which not only<br />

moved closer to the target time, but whose coefficients of variation decreased.<br />

<strong>The</strong> changes were inconsistent with several basic models of<br />

timing.<br />

• DECISION MAKING •<br />

(2054)<br />

Melioration Versus Maximization: Feedback- and Strategy-Driven<br />

Models of Adaptive Behavior. HANSJOERG NETH, CHRIS R. SIMS,<br />

& WAYNE D. GRAY, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute (sponsored by<br />

Wayne D. Gray)—Environments that confront cognitive agents with<br />

a choice between local and global gains often evoke a behavioral pattern<br />

known as melioration—a preference for immediate rewards over<br />

higher long-term gains (Herrnstein, 1997; Tunney & Shanks, 2002).<br />

We previously presented evidence that global feedback about expected<br />

and optimal gains does not enable decision makers to reliably<br />

overcome melioration in favor of maximization in a probabilistic<br />

binary-choice paradigm (Neth, Sims, & Gray, 2006). Treating the<br />

dilemma between melioration and maximization as a representational

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!