29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 2041–2047 Friday Noon<br />

portantly, this facilitation could only have occurred if people noticed<br />

the shared label, an example of category discovery via “similarity detection”<br />

as opposed to contrast or “difference detection.” This ability<br />

might compensate for the limitations of a purely failure-driven<br />

approach.<br />

(2041)<br />

Motivational Focus Interacts With Reward Structure in Category<br />

Learning. RUBY T. NADLER, JOHN PAUL MINDA, & LILY PEI-<br />

SHIUAN LIN, University of Western Ontario—Recent research suggests<br />

that categories are learned by multiple brain systems, and that<br />

situations that allow for greater cognitive flexibility will lead to better<br />

performance on rule-described categories that depend on an explicit<br />

system. We examined the effects of motivational focus (e.g.,<br />

promotion or prevention) and task reward structure (e.g., gain or loss)<br />

on participants’ ability to learn suboptimal or optimal rules. Two conditions<br />

featured a motivational focus that matched the reward structure<br />

of the task (promotion/gain and prevention/loss). Two conditions<br />

featured a motivation structure that did not match the reward structure<br />

of the task (promotion/loss and prevention/gain). We found that<br />

performance was generally better in the matching conditions than in<br />

the mismatching conditions and that participants in the matching conditions<br />

were more likely to learn complex, optimal rules. <strong>The</strong>se results<br />

suggest that the matching conditions allow for greater cognitive flexibility<br />

and support a multiple-systems account of category learning.<br />

(2042)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Incidental Learning of Within-Category Attribute Correlations<br />

in a One-Attribute Rule Visual Search Classification Paradigm.<br />

GUY L. LACROIX, Carleton University, GYSLAIN GIGUÈRE, University<br />

of Quebec, Montreal, GLEN HOWELL, Carleton University,<br />

& SERGE LAROCHELLE, University of Montreal—Giguère, Lacroix,<br />

and Larochelle (2007) showed that participants could incidentally learn<br />

within-category attribute correlations in a one-attribute rule classification<br />

task. Nevertheless, learning was limited. In this study, participants<br />

categorized stimuli in a one-attribute rule visual search classification<br />

paradigm. We hypothesized that the search component would<br />

increase learning. Two experiments were conducted. Both involved a<br />

classification training phase of 640 trials. <strong>The</strong> stimuli were six-shape<br />

displays that included a rule attribute and five diagnostic attributes.<br />

In Experiment 1, the rule attribute (and up to two other attributes)<br />

were removed at transfer. <strong>The</strong> results showed that several attributes<br />

(color, texture, and size) of varying diagnosticity were used to correctly<br />

classify the stimuli. In Experiment 2, attribute values were<br />

changed at transfer. Slower RTs were obtained when attribute values<br />

from conflicting categories were used. <strong>The</strong>se experiments provide evidence<br />

that within-category attribute correlations can be learned in a<br />

classification task without inference learning instructions.<br />

(2043)<br />

New Category Representations Are Biased by Implicit Perceptions.<br />

MICHAEL E. ROBERTS & ROBERT L. GOLDSTONE, Indiana<br />

University—Our study examines the effects of implicit perceptual representations<br />

when a participant first learns a category. Participants sequentially<br />

viewed pictures of two category members for familiar categories<br />

(e.g., dog, cow, airplane, etc.) and novel categories (artificial<br />

stimuli), and the second, primed member was followed by either a relevant<br />

or irrelevant story. After training on all categories in a set, participants<br />

completed a delayed matching test that showed a category<br />

label and then a picture of a category member or a member from another<br />

category. Participants responded significantly faster and more<br />

accurately for trials involving familiar categories, and we attribute this<br />

enhancement to well-learned category representations. However, for<br />

the novel category set, participants responded significantly faster to<br />

primed members that had been followed by relevant stories. This result<br />

suggests that participants perceptually invoked the primed member<br />

representation while reading the story, and that these automatic invocations<br />

biased the new category representation.<br />

75<br />

(2044)<br />

Contribution of the Declarative Memory System to Probabilistic<br />

Category Learning. JULIE SPICER, Columbia University, MURRAY<br />

GROSSMAN, University of Pennsylvania, & EDWARD E. SMITH,<br />

Columbia University—<strong>The</strong> multiple memory systems approach posits<br />

that different forms of knowledge are acquired and stored in qualitatively<br />

different ways. Evidence has suggested that probabilistic category<br />

learning, as assessed with the weather prediction (WP) task, can<br />

be supported by two systems: a system that acquires nondeclarative<br />

habit knowledge and a system that acquires declarative flexible knowledge.<br />

One focus of recent research has been to characterize the mechanisms<br />

through which each system contributes to behavior as information<br />

is acquired in parallel. In the present study, we have further<br />

characterized the contribution of the declarative system to WP learning.<br />

We show that individual differences in working memory relate<br />

positively to categorization accuracy and that patients with Alzheimer’s<br />

Disease do not reach an above-chance level of categorization accuracy,<br />

supporting a strong role for declarative learning in the WP task. Implications<br />

for the role of nondeclarative learning are also discussed.<br />

(2045)<br />

Category Learning, Binding, and the Medial Temporal Lobe: Evidence<br />

From Early Alzheimer’s Patients. JARED X. VAN SNELLEN-<br />

BERG & JANET METCALFE, Columbia University, MURRAY<br />

GROSSMAN, University of Pennsylvania, & EDWARD E. SMITH,<br />

Columbia University (sponsored by Edward E. Smith)—Despite considerable<br />

impairments in explicit memory, patients with medial temporal<br />

lobe (MTL) lesions and patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD)<br />

have been shown to have intact learning in a number of implicit learning<br />

paradigms, including category learning. Some recent evidence,<br />

however, suggests that “explicitness” per se may not determine the integrity<br />

of learning in individuals with a compromised MTL. One alternative<br />

is that a “binding” process, in which distinct elements of a<br />

stimulus or event become associated in memory, is critically mediated<br />

by MTL. In a test of this hypothesis, we showed that AD patients are<br />

at chance performance, and significantly worse than control participants,<br />

on an implicit two-category learning task that requires binding<br />

for successful performance.<br />

(2046)<br />

Modeling Process Differences in Implicit and Explicit Category<br />

Learning: A Symbolic-Connectionist Approach. LEONIDAS A. A.<br />

DOUMAS, Indiana University, & ROBERT G. MORRISON, Northwestern<br />

University—Much experimental evidence suggests both implicit<br />

(i.e., feature-based) and explicit (i.e., rule-based) mechanisms<br />

for learning categories, and these systems may compete and/or interact<br />

in different circumstances. However, most computational attempts<br />

to capture these processes typically rely on very different representational<br />

assumptions. For instance, models of implicit category learning<br />

are typically based on connectionist architectures consisting of<br />

networks of distributed units. In contrast, models of explicit category<br />

learning typically employ some type of symbolic architecture allowing<br />

for the flexibility characteristic of categories learned via rules. We<br />

present a model of category learning, DORA, that uses different learning<br />

mechanisms on a single distributed representation, allowing us to<br />

account for both implicit and explicit learning from a visual category<br />

learning task (Maddox, Ashby, & Bohil, 2003). We also successfully<br />

simulate differential working memory effects on implicit and explicit<br />

versions of this task as reported by Zeithamova and Maddox (2006).<br />

• ANIMAL COGNITION •<br />

(2047)<br />

Rats’ Memory for Event Duration: Effect of Postsample Cues to<br />

Remember and Forget. ANGELO SANTI, NEIL MCMILLAN, &<br />

PATRICK VAN ROOYEN, Wilfrid Laurier University—Rats were<br />

trained to discriminate 2 sec versus 8 sec of magazine light illumination<br />

by responding to either a stationary lever or a moving lever. Dur-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!