29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 2015–2021 Friday Noon<br />

Smith and Wheeldon (1999), sentence onset latencies were longer for<br />

sentences beginning with complex rather than simple phrases, consistent<br />

with a phrasal scope of planning. However, data from the name<br />

agreement and frequency manipulations indicated that planning beyond<br />

the initial phrase occurred when the nouns in the first phrase had<br />

high name agreement and high frequency. Thus, although the results<br />

are consistent with the phrase as a minimal scope, they indicate that<br />

scope may be extended when initial phrase words are easy to retrieve.<br />

(2015)<br />

Word Frequency Affects Subject–Verb Number Agreement.<br />

ERICA L. MIDDLETON & KATHRYN BOCK, University of Illinois,<br />

Urbana-Champaign—<strong>The</strong> traditional account of subject–verb number<br />

agreement (e.g., “<strong>The</strong> flag is/<strong>The</strong> flags are”) is that the grammatical<br />

number of the subject noun (e.g., singular “flag” or plural “flags”) dictates<br />

verb number. However, other factors have been found to influence<br />

agreement, including the number meanings of subject phrases<br />

and the plurality of nonsubject (local) nouns in the verb’s vicinity<br />

(e.g., “<strong>The</strong> flag near the ambassadors were colorful”), creating attraction.<br />

We investigated a third nongrammatical influence on agreement:<br />

the relative frequencies, or contrastiveness, of the singular and<br />

plural forms of local nouns. Participants heard and repeated preambles<br />

(e.g., “<strong>The</strong> flag near the ambassadors”), completing them as full<br />

sentences that included verbs. Verb number was sensitive to variations<br />

in contrastiveness, with plural attraction increasing as contrastiveness<br />

increased. This suggests that the implementation of agreement during<br />

language production is sensitive to competition among form or meaning<br />

related entries in the lexicon.<br />

(2016)<br />

Acoustic Analyses of the Sarcastic Tone of Voice. GINA M. CAUCCI,<br />

ROGER J. KREUZ, & EUGENE H. BUDER, University of Memphis—<br />

Sarcastic intent can be signaled by speakers in a variety of ways, such<br />

as by rolling one’s eyes. Other paralinguistic cues, such as tone of<br />

voice, can also cue listeners to a speaker’s sarcastic intentions (Cutler,<br />

1974; Roberts & Kreuz, 1994). However, the exact parameters that<br />

define this tone of voice have been the subject of some dispute (e.g.,<br />

Bryant & Fox Tree, 2005). This project investigated the acoustic properties<br />

of the sarcastic tone of voice. Participants were recorded in a<br />

conversational setting while engaged in tasks designed to naturally<br />

elicit sarcasm. <strong>The</strong> amount of common ground shared by conversational<br />

dyads was manipulated (friends vs. strangers). <strong>The</strong> pitch, duration,<br />

and amplitude for words intended sarcastically was measured,<br />

and compared with the same words when used literally. Preliminary<br />

data suggest that sarcastic statements differ from nonsarcastic statements<br />

on these acoustic parameters, and that shared common ground<br />

influences speakers’ use of sarcasm.<br />

(2017)<br />

Wernicke’s Aphasia: Exploring the Interface Between Language and<br />

Thought. PAUL D. MCGEOCH, DAVID BRANG, & VILAYANUR S.<br />

RAMACHANDRAN, University of California, San Diego—We studied<br />

patients with Wernicke’s aphasia to explore whether covert language<br />

is required for thinking. We found that: (1) Speech had most aspects<br />

of syntactic structure but no recursiveness. Contrary to popular belief,<br />

Broca’s area is not sufficient for this ability; it clearly also requires<br />

the Wernicke’s area (semantics) and, indeed, may be an aspect of<br />

many brain functions, such as self-awareness. (2) <strong>The</strong>y could lie nonverbally—implying<br />

a theory of other minds. (3) Additional evidence<br />

for Wernicke’s patients possessing a theory of other minds was given<br />

when one patient was shown a small cookie being placed in box A and<br />

a big one in box B. A and B were then hidden by a partition and a<br />

stooge was seen to go pick one of the cookies. <strong>The</strong> patient didn’t know<br />

which. <strong>The</strong> patient then chose box A, having inferred that the stooge<br />

must have (obviously) picked the large cookie.<br />

(2018)<br />

Online Writing Processes of Subject–Verb Agreement in Dutch–<br />

71<br />

English Bilinguals. JANET G. VAN HELL & MARIJKE MENSIES,<br />

Radboud University Nijmegen (sponsored by Janet G. van Hell)—We<br />

studied online processes of subject–verb agreement construction in<br />

written language production in bilinguals’ first and second language.<br />

Past research, studying errors in language production, shows that<br />

Dutch and English monolingual speakers differ in their sensitivity to<br />

the conceptual number of the sentential subject. We studied subject–verb<br />

agreement in Dutch–English bilinguals using a sentence fragment<br />

completion paradigm and contrasted conceptually multiple token<br />

fragments with single token fragments. <strong>The</strong> bilinguals wrote in their<br />

first (Dutch) or second (English) language. In addition to studying errors<br />

in language production (the dependent measure used in previous<br />

production studies), we examined online processes of writing using a<br />

digitizer tablet. Analyses of pause time patterns show longer pause durations<br />

after the head noun and before the verb in writing multiple<br />

token fragments in both languages. This suggests that conceptual information<br />

influences subject–verb agreement construction at an early<br />

stage, in first and second language writing.<br />

(2019)<br />

Orthographic Priming and Homophone Substitution Errors: Producing<br />

Similarly-Spelled Words Might Influence What You Right.<br />

KATHERINE K. WHITE, SAMANTHA M. GIBSON, & SARAH M.<br />

ZOLLER, College of Charleston, & LISE ABRAMS, University of<br />

Florida—Despite considerable research on language production errors<br />

involving speech, little research exists in the complementary domain<br />

of writing. Two experiments investigated the production of written<br />

homophone substitution errors, which occur when a contextually<br />

appropriate word (e.g., beech) is replaced with its homophone (e.g.,<br />

beach tree). Participants wrote auditorily presented sentences that included<br />

a contextually appropriate dominant or subordinate homophone<br />

(e.g., <strong>The</strong> teacher was most proud of the beech tree in his garden).<br />

Each homophone was preceded by a prime or unrelated word.<br />

Primes overlapped in phonology and orthography (teacher) or in orthography<br />

only (headmaster) with the contextually inappropriate homophone<br />

(beach). More errors occurred when the context elicited a<br />

subordinate homophone relative to a dominant homophone. Furthermore,<br />

both types of primes increased errors relative to unrelated<br />

words. <strong>The</strong>se results are discussed within dual route theories and suggest<br />

that recent exposure to shared graphemes, with or without shared<br />

phonology, increases homophone substitution errors.<br />

(2020)<br />

Lexical Influences on Nonword Spelling. TAMI J. PATTERSON &<br />

JOCELYN R. FOLK, Kent State University—Previous work has<br />

demonstrated that the processes dedicated to spelling familiar words<br />

(lexical process) and unfamiliar words (sublexical process) interact<br />

during spelling. We investigated two possible mechanisms underlying<br />

lexical influence on the sublexical system during nonword spelling: a<br />

dynamic and temporary reweighting of phoneme–grapheme probabilities<br />

in the sublexical system after exposure to a related word and<br />

residual activation of a previously heard word’s letters at the grapheme<br />

level. Participants listened to lists of intermixed words and nonwords<br />

and were required to spell only the nonwords. On a small percentage<br />

of trials the word preceding a target nonword rhymed with the nonword.<br />

We varied the interval between prime words and target nonwords<br />

(0 or 2 intervening items) and the neighborhood characteristics<br />

of the nonwords. We found evidence that both possible mechanisms<br />

contribute to lexical–sublexical interaction and that lexical neighbors<br />

influence nonword spelling. We discuss implications for models of the<br />

spelling system.<br />

• DIVIDED ATTENTION •<br />

(2021)<br />

Dividing Attention Between Two Different Categories and Locations.<br />

JUN I. KAWAHARA, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science<br />

and Technology, & YUKI YAMADA, Kyushu University—When two

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!