29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Friday Noon Posters 2008–2014<br />

speed (Prinzmetal, McCool, & Park, 2005). Uninformative gaze cues<br />

(Experiment 1) and arrow cues (Experiment 2) were presented centrally<br />

to participants in a peripheral letter-discrimination task. For<br />

both cue types, compatible cues were found to facilitate reaction time<br />

but not error rate, demonstrating that gaze and arrow cues affect response<br />

speed but not perceptual accuracy. This dissociation between<br />

reaction time and error effects indicates that both gaze and arrow cues<br />

generate reflexive shifts of attention.<br />

(2008)<br />

Predicting Preference From Fixations. MACKENZIE G. GLAHOLT<br />

& EYAL M. REINGOLD, University of Toronto, Mississauga—We<br />

extended work by Shimojo, Simion, Shimojo, and Scheier (2003), and<br />

measured the strength of the association between fixation duration<br />

and preference. Participants selected the most attractive of eight stimuli<br />

arranged in a grid. Eye fixations correlated with selection, as well<br />

as with explicit attractiveness ratings, and also predicted selection on<br />

a trial-by-trial basis. By ranking features on the basis of fixation time,<br />

we were able to successfully predict participants’ preferences for<br />

novel feature combinations in a two-alternative forced choice task.<br />

<strong>The</strong>oretical and applied implications are discussed.<br />

(2009)<br />

Visual Marking of Emotionally Valenced Face Stimuli. ELISABETH<br />

BLAGROVE & DERRICK G. WATSON, University of Warwick—<br />

Visual marking (Watson & Humphreys, 1997) increases search efficiency<br />

for new items via deprioritization of old (previewed) items already<br />

in the field (the preview benefit). We examined whether a preview<br />

benefit arises with schematic face stimuli and the extent to which<br />

their emotional valence modulates these effects. Three experiments<br />

investigated the ability to ignore neutral, positive, or negative valenced<br />

faces. In all cases, a partial preview benefit was obtained and negative<br />

faces were detected more efficiently than positive faces. However,<br />

there was a trend for previewed negative faces to be less easily suppressed<br />

than were positive faces. <strong>The</strong> results extend our understanding<br />

of when preferential processing of negative valenced stimuli occurs<br />

and are interpreted in terms of the ecological properties and<br />

constraints of time-based visual selection.<br />

• LANGUAGE PRODUCTION •<br />

(2010)<br />

Sentence Production in Monologue and Dialogue: <strong>The</strong> Scope of<br />

Planning. BENJAMIN SWETS, RICHARD J. GERRIG, MATTHEW E.<br />

JACOVINA, & WILLIAM G. WENZEL, Stony Brook University (sponsored<br />

by Richard J. Gerrig)—Little is known about the differences between<br />

planning speech in monologue (speech without an addressee)<br />

versus dialogue (speech with an addressee). Here, we test the hypothesis<br />

that the pressure on speakers to respond in a timely fashion<br />

should render high-level utterance planning more incremental (i.e.,<br />

advanced planning should be less extensive) in dialogue than in monologue.<br />

In our study, participants produced brief utterances to describe<br />

visual displays. We complicated utterance planning by including tangram<br />

figures that prohibited easy lexicalization. Participants completed<br />

the task in circumstances of monologue or dialogue, and also<br />

under circumstances of natural or explicit time pressure. We examined<br />

the time course with which participants planned upcoming tangrams<br />

and proceeded with articulation as a function of addressee presence<br />

and time pressure. We suggest that speech production is quite adaptable<br />

to its circumstances, and that its resources are strategically deployed<br />

depending on the environment in which they are used.<br />

(2011)<br />

Assessing the Role of Morphological Marking in Subject–Verb<br />

Gender Agreement: Evidence From Hebrew. AVITAL DEUTSCH &<br />

MAYA DANK, Hebrew University of Jerusalem—<strong>The</strong> present study<br />

investigated the role of morphological markedness on the implementation<br />

of subject–predicate agreement in language production. This<br />

70<br />

study was conducted in Hebrew, using a sentence completion task, and<br />

focused on gender agreement. In Hebrew, masculine forms are usually<br />

morphologically unmarked, whereas feminine forms are morphologically<br />

marked. Although it is generally consistent, the system<br />

includes examples of feminine forms which are not inflected for feminine<br />

suffixes—that is, unmarked feminine forms. Furthermore, there<br />

are particular suffixes that denote masculine and feminine plurals.<br />

However, there are many examples of masculine plurals which are inflected<br />

irregularly, by a feminine suffix. We took advantage of these<br />

characteristics to manipulate morphological factors in the process of<br />

agreement production. <strong>The</strong> results suggest that gender agreement cannot<br />

be explained solely in terms of overt inflectional morphology;<br />

however, it is subjected to morphophonological influence. Furthermore,<br />

the results suggest the possible involvement of derivational<br />

morphology in syntactic agreement.<br />

(2012)<br />

Working Memory and Oral Fluency During Spontaneous Language<br />

Production. IRENA O’BRIEN & DEBRA A. TITONE, McGill University,<br />

DAVID MCFARLAND, University of Montreal, CAROLINE<br />

PALMER, McGill University, & NATALIE PHILLIPS, Concordia<br />

University—Models of spontaneous language production propose<br />

that speech planning occurs incrementally (Bock, 1982; Levelt, 1989).<br />

Within this conceptual framework, working memory would be crucial<br />

to appropriate speech planning. <strong>The</strong> present study investigated<br />

whether individual differences in working memory were related to<br />

various aspects of speech production. Working memory measures,<br />

using a modified listening span (Waters & Caplan, 1996) and backward<br />

digit span tasks, and spontaneous speech recordings were obtained<br />

from 44 native English speakers. In addition to final word recall,<br />

the listening span task also measured sentence judgment times.<br />

Multiple regression analyses revealed that speakers with higher backward<br />

digit span performance spoke with longer runs and shorter<br />

pauses. In addition, speakers with shorter sentence judgment times in<br />

the listening span task spoke faster and exhibited fewer hesitations.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results offer preliminary evidence that working memory may<br />

facilitate language production by allowing more efficient incremental<br />

planning.<br />

(2013)<br />

Working Memory and the Production of Metaphors. RUSSELL<br />

PIERCE, University of California, Riverside, & CANDACE STUTE,<br />

THERESA FRASER, MICHELLE RIFI, ELIZABETH UBIDES, &<br />

DAN CHIAPPE, California State University, Long Beach—We examined<br />

the role of working memory (WM) in metaphor production.<br />

We administered to participants the operation span, word span, and<br />

listening span tasks as measures of WM. For the metaphor production<br />

task, participants were given topics (e.g., “billboards ____”) plus a<br />

property that had to be attributed (e.g., “billboards are something noticeable<br />

and unattractive”). For each item, they had to provide a vehicle<br />

term (e.g., “warts”) that can be used to attribute the property. Difficulty<br />

of the items was manipulated by varying their conventionality<br />

(i.e., whether the properties are conventionally associated with a vehicle<br />

term) and aptness (i.e., whether the properties being attributed<br />

are a good fit with the topic). We recorded time to generate vehicles,<br />

and rated the quality of the vehicles. WM measures predicted the quality<br />

of metaphor vehicles produced regardless of difficulty, but did not<br />

predict time to generate those vehicles.<br />

(2014)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Phrase As a Minimal Planning Scope in Sentence Production.<br />

JASON E. CROWTHER & RANDI C. MARTIN, Rice University—<br />

In a sentence production study, subjects described the arrangement of<br />

three stationary pictures using sentences such as “<strong>The</strong> foot is above<br />

the candle and the faucet” or “<strong>The</strong> foot and the candle are above the<br />

faucet.” Complexity of the initial phrase (i.e., one vs. two nouns) and<br />

word frequency and name agreement for the first two nouns were manipulated.<br />

Onset latencies for each noun were measured. Replicating

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!