29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 1110–1116 Thursday Evening<br />

(1110)<br />

Older Adults Search Working Memory As Efficiently As Younger<br />

Adults: Evidence From Four Different Memory Scanning Conditions.<br />

ELKE B. LANGE & PAUL VERHAEGHEN, Georgia Institute<br />

of Technology (sponsored by Paul Verhaeghen)—In two experiments,<br />

we compared performance of young and old adults in a serial recognition<br />

task. In this task the visual presentation of N digits at N locations<br />

was followed by N recognition probes (N = 3, 4, 5). Participants<br />

had to decide whether the probe digit occurred at the same location<br />

as in the encoding phase. We probed items predictably (forward, backward,<br />

a learned irregular pattern) and randomly. RT is independent of<br />

N only when the probe recognition order matches the encoding order;<br />

otherwise RT increased linearly with set size, with the learned irregular<br />

probing yielding the largest slope. After logarithmic transformation<br />

of the data and subtraction of sensorimotor RT, no age � slope<br />

interactions were found, showing that the search efficiency of older<br />

adults is a simple multiplicative function of that of the young.<br />

(1111)<br />

Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Other<br />

Cognitive Abilities Influence L2 Reading Comprehension With<br />

Missing Information. MICHAEL BUNTING, SCOTT A. WEEMS,<br />

DIMITRIOS K. DONAVOS, ELIZABETH ROGLER, & HENK J.<br />

HAARMANN, University of Maryland—Missing words (i.e., completeness)<br />

and missing background information (i.e., context) can adversely<br />

impact readability and comprehension, especially for readers<br />

with lower language proficiency. Our objective was to identify the role<br />

of individual differences in salient cognitive factors in L2 reading<br />

comprehension when completeness and context vary. L2 reading comprehension<br />

skills of 82 adult foreign-language professionals (L1: English;<br />

L2: Spanish acquired post-critical period; lower- to upper-intermediate<br />

L2 proficiency) were assessed in tests of narrative and<br />

conversation comprehension, inferential reading, and reading with<br />

spatial reasoning. Subjects made inferences about missing words,<br />

missing context, or the spatial arrangement of items, or avoided misleading<br />

inferences. L1 working memory (WM), L1 and L2 processing<br />

speeds, reasoning ability, L1 reading ability, creativity, and situation<br />

memory were assessed. Preliminary findings indicate that WM<br />

and L1 reading ability are the strongest correlates of L2 reading comprehension<br />

when drawing inferences is difficult.<br />

(1112)<br />

Domain-Specific Proactive Interference Effects Are Predicted by Impairments<br />

in Short-Term Memory Capacity. LAURA H. F. BARDE<br />

& MYRNA F. SCHWARTZ, Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute,<br />

& SHARON L. THOMPSON-SCHILL, University of Pennsylvania<br />

(sponsored by Myrna F. Schwartz)—To date, studies regarding mechanisms<br />

of proactive interference (PI) in working memory have focused<br />

primarily on the integrity of control processes (i.e., inhibition,<br />

selection). In a patient study, we took an alternative approach, wherein<br />

variation in domain-specific short-term memory (STM) capacity was<br />

correlated with resistance to semantic or phonological PI (sPI and<br />

pPI). Twelve patients with left hemisphere stroke performed a modified<br />

item-recognition task (Hamilton & Martin, 2006; Monsell, 1978)<br />

designed to elicit sPI or pPI from list items located one, two, or three<br />

trials back. Following a z-transformation of each patient’s interference<br />

measure, referencing an age-matched control group (n = 20), we<br />

found domain-specific patterns of PI. That is, impairments in semantic<br />

STM were associated with sPI, but not with pPI. <strong>The</strong> reverse effect<br />

was found in patients with phonological maintenance deficits. We<br />

interpret the results with regard to extant models of STM impairment<br />

and accounts of PI resolution.<br />

(1113)<br />

Effects of Phonological Neighborhood Size and Biphone Frequency<br />

on Serial Recall of Words. LINDA MORTENSEN & RANDI C.<br />

MARTIN, Rice University—Immediate serial recall is more accurate<br />

for lists composed of nonwords with many than with few phonologi-<br />

67<br />

cal neighbors and for lists of nonwords with high than with low biphone<br />

frequency (Thorn & Frankish, 2005). We investigated the independent<br />

effects of phonological neighborhood size (Experiments 1<br />

and 2) and biphone frequency (Experiments 3 and 4) on immediate serial<br />

recall of high- and low-frequency words. When the lists were sampled<br />

from a closed set of words, neighborhood size had no effect.<br />

When the lists were from an open set, a facilitatory effect was observed<br />

but only for low-frequency words. Biphone frequency facilitated<br />

recall of both high- and low-frequency words in both closed and<br />

open sets. <strong>The</strong>se results confirm that phonological neighborhood effects<br />

on recall of words exist but suggest that the effect is confined to<br />

low-frequency words. <strong>The</strong> influence of lexical and sublexical phonological<br />

knowledge on short-term memory for words is discussed.<br />

(1114)<br />

Phonological and Orthographic Similarity Affects Verbal Short-<br />

Term Memory for Chinese Characters. CHE-YING LAI &<br />

DENISE H. WU, National Central University, & DAISY L. HUNG<br />

& OVID J.-L. TZENG, National Yang-Ming University (sponsored by<br />

Ovid J.-L. Tzeng)—Prominent theories of short-term memory (STM)<br />

emphasize the importance of phonology to retaining verbal materials,<br />

whereas orthography is only considered supplementary when phonology<br />

is not available or useful. However, when retaining Chinese characters,<br />

whose correspondence between phonology and orthography is<br />

relatively irregular in comparison with most alphabetic languages, orthographic<br />

contribution might be more likely to express. We manipulated<br />

the phonological and orthographic similarity between Chinese<br />

characters independently in three experiments. <strong>The</strong> contribution of<br />

phonology was consistently observed across three experiments, indicating<br />

the dominant role of this kind of information in verbal STM.<br />

More importantly, the contribution of orthography was also demonstrated<br />

in both phonologically dissimilar and similar characters after<br />

the strategy of focusing on other aspects of Chinese characters was<br />

prevented. Our findings suggest that orthography is also a crucial<br />

component of verbal STM for retaining Chinese characters, even<br />

when the phonological information is readily available.<br />

• IMPLICIT LEARNING AND MEMORY •<br />

(1115)<br />

Memory Consolidation During Rapid Serial Visual Presentation.<br />

ANDREAS BREUER & MICHAEL E. J. MASSON, University of<br />

Victoria, ANNA-LISA COHEN, Yeshiva University, & D. STEPHEN<br />

LINDSAY, University of Victoria (sponsored by Michael E. J. Masson)—<br />

It has been claimed that for a briefly viewed object to be encoded into<br />

long-term memory, the viewing episode must undergo a process of<br />

memory consolidation beyond the initial processing needed to identify<br />

the object. Previous demonstrations have supported the conclusion<br />

that memory consolidation can take several hundred milliseconds.<br />

Contrary to this claim, we provide evidence that pictures shown<br />

for only 75 msec in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) format<br />

with no motivation for processing beyond immediate identification do<br />

exhibit improved performance in an indirect test of memory (masked<br />

object identification), even when performance is at chance in a recognition<br />

memory test. We also determined that the memory episodes<br />

formed from the RSVP sequences were partly perceptual, as shown<br />

by better performance for exact replications of the studied picture relative<br />

to mirror reversals, and partly conceptual, as shown by crossmodal<br />

priming.<br />

(1116)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Role of Color in Object Memory. TOBY J. LLOYD-JONES &<br />

KAZUYO NAKABAYASHI, University of Kent—We examined the<br />

effects of repetition priming, object format (pictures vs. words), color<br />

format (color patches vs. words), and color transformation between<br />

study and test (from correctly colored to incorrectly colored and vice<br />

versa) on colored-object decision performance (i.e., deciding whether<br />

an object was correctly colored) using diagnostically colored objects

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!