29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Posters 1083–1089 Thursday Evening<br />

practice and performance. By means of a linear mixed models analysis,<br />

we showed that throughout chess players’ careers, a strong relation<br />

was found between deliberate practice and chess ratings. Moreover,<br />

differences between persisters and later dropouts were the result<br />

of lower engagement in deliberate practice, from an early point on.<br />

This rejects the possibility that the relation between deliberate practice<br />

and performance only holds for those who eventually reach the<br />

top. Finally, the performance difference between males and females<br />

could mainly be attributed to less deliberate practice by females.<br />

(1083)<br />

Learning the Relations Between Multiple Inputs and Outputs:<br />

Integration Effects in a Diagnostic Task. JON TALL, ROBERT<br />

MATHEWS, SEAN LANE, & BILL SALLAS, Louisiana State University—In<br />

real-world situations, it is often difficult to figure out what<br />

“works.” We often have multiple behavioral options and multiple ways<br />

that we could measure the impact of our behavior. In three experiments,<br />

we modeled this complexity using a managerial task that involved<br />

learning the effect of multiple inputs (interventions) on multiple<br />

performance measures of employees. Participants “met” with<br />

each employee to improve their performance and to learn which interventions<br />

were most helpful overall. Later, on separate tests, they<br />

prescribed the best intervention for each participant and indicated the<br />

impact of each intervention on each performance measure. <strong>The</strong> critical<br />

manipulation was whether a key variable had a negative side effect<br />

or not. <strong>The</strong> major finding was that, although participants appeared<br />

to integrate their knowledge of the side effect into their<br />

decisions (by prescribing it less), they nevertheless did not indicate<br />

any knowledge of its impact when explicitly queried about it.<br />

• IMAGERY AND SPATIAL COGNITION •<br />

(1084)<br />

Quantitative and Qualitative Gender Differences in Learning<br />

Large Spaces. ANN EKECHUKWU, Vanderbilt University, JULIA<br />

SLUZENSKI, Richard Stockton College of New Jersey, & TIMOTHY P.<br />

MCNAMARA, Vanderbilt University (sponsored by Timothy P.<br />

McNamara)—Males and females walked a straight path through a field<br />

while memorizing the locations of surrounding objects. About 10 min<br />

later they performed a computer task requiring them to recall the interobject<br />

angles. Males had lower angular error than females, suggesting<br />

that the earlier finding was not due to the context of the baseball<br />

field. Males were also less likely than females to use a verbal strategy<br />

during learning. In addition, we investigated whether performance on<br />

the field task was related to performance on nonnavigational spatial<br />

tasks and to verbal ability. For males, performance on the field task was<br />

generally related to other spatial abilities, whereas for females this<br />

finding was not as strong. In contrast to males, female performance<br />

was related to verbal ability. Collectively, these findings suggest that<br />

males rely more on quantitative spatial coding than do females, and<br />

that females rely more on verbal spatial coding than do males.<br />

(1085)<br />

Factors Affecting Object–Object Information in Spatial Representations<br />

II. JESSE SARGENT, STEPHEN C. DOPKINS, & JOHN W.<br />

PHILBECK, George Washington University—Human subjects learned<br />

the location of four objects around them in a room. However, visual<br />

access was restricted during preview such that a particular adjacent<br />

pair of the four objects (between-image pair) could never be seen together<br />

from the same point of view. Participants were then blindfolded<br />

and underwent passive whole-body rotations, pointing to the objects<br />

after each rotation. <strong>The</strong> error in locating one object relative to another<br />

was greater for the between-image pair than for the within-image pairs<br />

that could be viewed together during preview. Between- and withinimage<br />

pairs were matched for angular separation. <strong>The</strong> results are interpreted<br />

as suggesting that in representations of detailed spatial information<br />

about remembered, immediate room-sized environments,<br />

certain objects “bind” together more tightly than others. Possible al-<br />

63<br />

locentric characteristics of such “bound” object representations and<br />

the role of visual imagery are considered.<br />

(1086)<br />

Similarity Between Room Layouts Causes Orientation-Specific<br />

Sensorimotor Interference in To-Be-Imagined Perspective Switches.<br />

BERNHARD E. RIECKE & TIMOTHY P. MCNAMARA, Vanderbilt<br />

University—May (2004) suggested that the difficulty of imagined perspective<br />

switches is partially caused by interference between the sensorimotor<br />

(actual) and to-be-imagined orientation. Here, we demonstrate<br />

a similar interference, even if participants are in a remote room<br />

and don’t know their physical orientation with respect to the to-beimagined<br />

orientation. Participants learned 15 target objects located in<br />

an office from one orientation (0º, 120º, or 240º). Participants were<br />

blindfolded and disoriented before being wheeled to an empty test<br />

room of similar geometry. Participants were seated facing 0º, 120º, or<br />

240º, and were asked to perform judgments of relative direction (e.g.,<br />

imagine facing “pen,” point to “phone”). Performance was facilitated<br />

when participants’ to-be-imagined orientation in the learning room<br />

was aligned with the corresponding orientation in the test room. This<br />

suggests that merely being in an empty room of similar geometry can<br />

be sufficient to automatically reanchor one’s representation and thus<br />

produce orientation-specific interference.<br />

(1087)<br />

Inferring Cross Sections: Benefits of Spatial Visualization Training.<br />

CHERYL A. COHEN & MARY HEGARTY, University of California,<br />

Santa Barbara (sponsored by Mary Hegarty)—We examined the benefits<br />

of training with virtual objects on a spatial visualization task that<br />

involves identifying the cross section of a geometric solid. In Experiment<br />

1, a novel 30-item multiple choice test was developed to measure<br />

individual differences in this task. Performance was related to orientation<br />

of the cutting plane (orthogonal or oblique) and to spatial ability.<br />

Errors indicated that low-spatial participants often failed to shift mental<br />

perspective to the requisite view orientation of the cross section. In<br />

Experiments 2 and 3, low-spatial participants were trained by allowing<br />

them to pass a plane through a virtual object and observe the resulting<br />

cross section. This training lead to large gains in performance<br />

of trained individuals in comparison with controls. Performance improved<br />

on the trained objects and transferred to novel objects. <strong>The</strong> results<br />

suggested that individuals learned to recognize specific cross sectional<br />

shapes and to assume the correct view orientation.<br />

(1088)<br />

Mitigating Effects of Spatial Ability on Mental and Manual Rotation.<br />

ANDREW T. STULL, MARY HEGARTY, & RICHARD E. MAYER,<br />

University of California, Santa Barbara—In two experiments, individuals<br />

of differing spatial ability were measured on response time,<br />

orientation matching, and path directness as they manipulated a virtual<br />

3-D object in two conditions, with or without orientation references<br />

(ORs). In Experiment 1, participants attempted to quickly and<br />

accurately match 40 targets randomly rotated around 40 unique, noncanonical<br />

axes. In Experiment 2, participants attempted to match 44<br />

targets systematically rotated around the three canonical axes or a<br />

noncanonical axis. In both experiments, the OR group responded significantly<br />

faster, more accurately, and more directly than the non-OR<br />

group. Performance was correlated with spatial ability, and effects of<br />

spatial ability were mitigated in the OR group. <strong>The</strong>se results suggest<br />

that orientation references may help individuals mentally and manually<br />

manipulate complex objects. Furthermore, it may be possible to<br />

alleviate difficulties of low-spatial individuals by providing orientation<br />

references.<br />

• SPATIAL MEMORY •<br />

(1089)<br />

Spatial Ability and Working Memory: Just and Carpenter (1985) Revisited.<br />

DALE KLOPFER, JEREMY ATHY, & LAURA LEVENTHAL,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!