S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Thursday Evening Posters 1062–1068 (1062) Evidence of Spontaneous Retrieval for Suspended but Not Completed Intentions. MICHAEL K. SCULLIN, Washington University, & GILLES O. EINSTEIN, Furman University—McDaniel and Einstein (2007) argue that prospective memories can be retrieved through spontaneous retrieval processes that respond to the presence of a target event. The present research investigated whether spontaneous retrieval processes continue to be activated after the completion of a prospective memory task. In two experiments, participants performed image-rating phases with a prospective memory task (press the “Q” key in presence of a target) embedded in one phase. Next, participants were told that their intention was either completed or suspended. Participants then performed a lexical decision task in which each target (and matched control) item appeared five times. Both experiments revealed slower response times to target items in comparison with control items when the intention was suspended but not when the intention was completed, thereby providing evidence for spontaneous retrieval only in the suspended condition. These initial results suggest that spontaneous retrieval processes are quickly deactivated following completion of an intention. (1063) Attentional Manipulations Affect Focal and Nonfocal Prospective Memory Cues Differently. GABRIEL I. COOK, Claremont McKenna College, J. THADEUS MEEKS & RICHARD L. MARSH, University of Georgia, & GILLES O. EINSTEIN, Furman University—Event-based prospective memories involve retrieving intentions in the presence of an environmental cue. Such cues vary in their specificity, and consequently, the degree to which they evoke retrieval of the previously established intention. According to the multiprocess view of prospective memory, very specific cues (i.e., focal cues) are detected relatively automatically, whereas less specific cues (i.e., nonfocal cues) require more attentional resources to be devoted to intention completion to achieve the same level of performance. Three experiments used three different manipulations of reducing the attention available for intention completion in order to demonstrate that in all three cases the detection of nonfocal cues was more severely impacted than the detection of focal cues. The manipulations included level of effort required to perform the task in which cues were embedded, a response signal procedure, and a random number divided attention task. Thus, not all event-based cues require the same resources to be detected. (1064) Effects of Very Long-Term Penetrating Head Injury on Laboratory Prospective Memory. LIA KVAVILASHVILI, University of Hertfordshire, & VANESSA RAYMONT & JORDAN GRAFMAN, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke—Research on patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI) has shown that they experience deficits in remembering both event- and time-based prospective memory tasks (remembering to do something in response to an event or at a particular time in the future, respectively). However, very long-term effects of TBI on prospective memory have not been examined. The present study assessed 199 Vietnam veterans with penetrating head injury (PHI) and 55 matched controls on three laboratory prospective memory tasks embedded within a general knowledge question answering task: a time-based task, and two event-based tasks with distinctive or nondistinctive target events. Performance of the control group on these tasks was not reliably better than that of the group with long-term PHI. Prospective memory performance will also be examined in relation to volume, hemisphere, and localization of the damage. (1065) Instructions to Complete or Forget an Intention Influence Stroop Performance. ANNA-LISA COHEN, Yeshiva University, & D. STEPHEN LINDSAY & JUSTIN KANTNER, University of Victoria—Research on the “intention superiority effect” (e.g., Goshke & Kuhl, 1993) revealed that material from scripts that are to be performed later by the participant are processed faster than is material 60 from a neutral script. Cohen, Dixon, and Lindsay (2005) demonstrated an “intention interference effect” for both young and older adults in which performance was slower for critical items belonging to an intention that participants intended to carry out versus critical items belonging to an intention that did not have to be executed. In a new line of studies, we examine the effect of critical item order. Because we used complex intentions that contained three critical items, we could examine how the superiority of the intention-related material builds across items. The results suggest that the first critical item acts as a reminder (showing no interference) and causes the remaining items to be retrieved reflexively, leading to Stroop interference. (1066) Effects of Delay and Type of Processing on Prospective Memory. KATHRYN A. HUDSON & DAWN M. MCBRIDE, Illinois State University—Prospective memory (PM) has been defined as the process of remembering future events. Multiprocess theory (McDaniel & Einstein, 2000) proposes that PM performance can be more automatic when the PM and ongoing tasks are highly associated (i.e., focal PM cues are presented), but utilizes more controlled processes when the PM and ongoing tasks are not highly associated (i.e., nonfocal PM cues are presented). The present study examined how the delay between PM task instruction and PM cue presentation affects PM task performance with focal and nonfocal PM cues. As reported in previous studies (e.g., Einstein & McDaniel, 2005), responses to the ongoing task were faster in focal PM cue conditions than in nonfocal PM cue conditions. In addition, delay effects on PM task performance differed for focal and nonfocal conditions. The results of the present study support multiprocess theory descriptions of PM task performance. • RECALL • (1067) Recalling the List Before the Last: Context Isolation, Context Retrieval, and Filtering Retrieved Memories. YOONHEE JANG & DAVID E. HUBER, University of California, San Diego—A series of experiments used the list before the last free recall paradigm, which manipulated the list length of both target and intervening lists to index the degree of interference from each list. Correct target list recall was only affected by the target list length when participants engaged in recall between the lists, whereas there was an effect of both list lengths with other activities. This suggests that the act of recalling drives context change, isolating the target list from interference. Incorrect recall of intervening list items was affected only by the length of the intervening list when recall occurred between the lists, but was otherwise affected by both list lengths. A multinomial model of sampling and recovery was implemented to assess the adequacy of this account. These results suggest a more nuanced account of retroactive interference that includes context isolation, context retrieval, and the ability to filter retrieved memories. (1068) Memory by Sampling. CAROLINE MORIN & GORDON D. A. BROWN, University of Warwick—According to temporal distinctiveness models of memory such as SIMPLE (Brown, Neath, & Chater, 2007), memory items’ temporal locations are represented as points on a temporal dimension in multidimensional psychological space. Such models do not provide a natural account of item presentation duration effects or rehearsal effects. Here, we explore predictions of a modified time-based model (memory by sampling) in which the temporal extension of items is explicitly represented. In Experiment 1, participants viewed lists of words whose presentation times followed either a U-shaped or an inverted U-shaped distribution. Participants overtly rehearsed each item during its presentation. In Experiment 2, presentation durations were random. Item recall increased systematically with item presentation time, especially in delayed free recall (Experiments 3 and 4). The results were as predicted by the memory by sampling model.

Posters 1069–1075 Thursday Evening (1069) Encoding Strategy and the Testing Effect in Free Recall. PETER P. J. L. VERKOEIJEN, Erasmus University Rotterdam, PETER F. DE- LANEY, University of North Carolina, Greensboro, & REMY M. J. P. RIKERS, Erasmus University Rotterdam (sponsored by Remy M. J. P. Rikers)—The testing effect refers to the phenomenon that taking an intermediate test benefits long-term retention more than restudying the material does. In the present study, we investigated the interaction between encoding strategy, retention interval, and testing effects. Participants studied a word list using a story strategy or a rehearsal strategy. Subsequently, they restudied the list or they received an intermediate free-recall test. Then, half of the participants received a final test after 5 min, whereas the other half received the final test after 1 week. When the final test was administered after 5 min, restudying yielded a better memory performance than testing. This held true for the story and the rehearsal strategy. Conversely, when the final test was given after 1 week, a testing effect was obtained in the story-strategy condition, whereas a reversed testing was found in the rehearsal-strategy condition. The theoretical and the practical implications of these findings are discussed. (1070) The Effects of Effort After Meaning on Recall. FRANKLIN M. ZAROMB & HENRY L. ROEDIGER III, Washington University (sponsored by Todd Braver)—Three experiments examined free recall of ambiguous sentences with and without corresponding cues to facilitate comprehension using Auble and Franks’s (1977) paradigm. Sentences were either studied without cues, with cues provided immediately beforehand, or with cues following a 2- or 6-sec interval delay. When the conditions were manipulated within subjects, puzzling over the meaning of sentences for several seconds prior to receiving the cue to aid comprehension enhanced veridical recall and reduced false recall. The memorial advantage associated with increasing the sentencecue delay did not occur when the cue presentation conditions were arranged in homogeneous sentence-blocks or manipulated between subjects. However, when instructions were changed to direct subjects to try to discern the meaning of sentences prior to receiving a cue, recall improved in both the between-subjects and blocked-sentence designs. Effort after meaning improves recall. (1071) Episodic Cuing: More Information Makes Worse Memory. ANDREA K. TAMPLIN & GABRIEL A. RADVANSKY, University of Notre Dame (sponsored by Gabriel A. Radvansky)—In three experiments, people were trained on general information, with accompanying unique episodic information, to choose an appropriate factory task or filing procedure. The learning task emphasized both the general and the episodic knowledge. After training, people were given task numbers/names, and were asked to recall the general information associated with these tasks. Findings suggest that while episodic cues aided memory in some cases, in others they made memory worse. Memory was generally improved with episodic cues when the traces were relatively weak, and harmed when memory traces were stronger. These results are interpreted in the context of theories of focused and competitive memory retrieval. Specifically, providing an episodic cue may misdirect retrieval processes away from portions of memory containing the needed information, and there may even be suppression of noncued information. • WORKING MEMORY • (1072) Depth of Processing Differentially Affects Working Memory and Long-Term Memory. NATHAN S. ROSE, JOEL MYERSON, HENRY L. ROEDIGER III, & SANDRA HALE, Washington University—Two experiments compared the effects of depth of processing at encoding on working memory and long-term memory. Experiment 1 examined working memory performance on the levels-of-processing (LOP) 61 span task, a newly developed complex span procedure which involves processing to-be-remembered words on the basis of either their visual, phonological, or semantic characteristics. Using the LOP span task, Experiment 2 compared the effect of depth of processing at encoding on immediate serial recall with its effect on subsequent delayed recognition of the same items. Taken together, the results of the two experiments show that depth of processing has a minimal effect on working memory, but that even in the absence of an effect on immediate recall, there is a clear LOP effect on long-term memory for the same items. These results are consistent with previous findings showing that working memory and long-term memory are fundamentally separable systems and provide strong support for dual-store memory models. (1073) The Utilization of Domain Knowledge to Expand Working Memory Capacity. TRAVIS R. RICKS & JENNIFER WILEY, University of Illinois, Chicago (sponsored by Jennifer Wiley)—To investigate how expertise improves working memory capacity (WMC), we completed a set of studies that extended the Fincher-Kiefer et al. (1988) findings that domain knowledge increased scores on domain-related span tasks, which we replicate in our Experiment 1. In our subsequent studies, we isolated whether the storage or processing components of span tasks were related to baseball. In Experiment 2, processing was related, but storage items were not. No effects were seen due to domain knowledge. In Experiment 3, storage was related but processing was not. Again there was no effect for knowledge. Only Experiment 4, where the storage items were related to baseball, and participants were explicitly informed of the baseball theme, found an effect for knowledge. This suggests that experts expand their WMC by utilizing domainrelated retrieval structures, but that they need to be aware of the relation between to-be-remembered items and their prior knowledge. (1074) The Negative Impact of Prior Reflective Selection is Sometimes Due to Target Enhancement. JULIE A. HIGGINS & MARCIA K. JOHNSON, Yale University—Briefly thinking of one item from an active set of semantically related items reduces accessibility of the nonselected items during subsequent processing. We test whether this reduced accessibility is due to target enhancement or to distractor suppression. Participants read aloud a word set containing three related category exemplars (one high, one medium, and one low associate). Then they either reread or thought of (refreshed) one of the words. Participants read the original set again before refreshing a second word from the set. Response times to refresh the second word were longer having just refreshed versus having just reread a word from the set. This increase was larger when the first word refreshed was a higher associate than when it was a lower associate. This pattern of results suggests that the negative impact of prior refreshing is due to enhanced activation of the refreshed item and not inhibition of the nonrefreshed items. (1075) Do Working Memories Decay? MARC G. BERMAN, JOHN JONIDES, LEE I. NEWMAN, & DEREK E. NEE, University of Michigan (sponsored by John Jonides)—We present data from a series of experiments exploring decay and interference in working memory. Utilizing an item-recognition task, we examined the degree to which previously seen stimuli interfered with item-recognition performance on current memoranda. The amount of time that separated previously seen items from current memoranda was manipulated from 2 to 14 sec to determine whether working memories do in fact decay with the sheer passage of time. In addition, the amount of proactive interference (PI) that was aggregated over the course of the experiment was varied between studies to determine whether the amount of aggregated PI interacted with the amount of time-based decay. Lastly, we analyzed how the number of intervening trials modulated the interfering ability of previously seen items. Our preliminary results

Thursday Evening Posters 1062–1068<br />

(1062)<br />

Evidence of Spontaneous Retrieval for Suspended but Not Completed<br />

Intentions. MICHAEL K. SCULLIN, Washington University, &<br />

GILLES O. EINSTEIN, Furman University—McDaniel and Einstein<br />

(2007) argue that prospective memories can be retrieved through<br />

spontaneous retrieval processes that respond to the presence of a target<br />

event. <strong>The</strong> present research investigated whether spontaneous retrieval<br />

processes continue to be activated after the completion of a<br />

prospective memory task. In two experiments, participants performed<br />

image-rating phases with a prospective memory task (press the “Q”<br />

key in presence of a target) embedded in one phase. Next, participants<br />

were told that their intention was either completed or suspended. Participants<br />

then performed a lexical decision task in which each target<br />

(and matched control) item appeared five times. Both experiments revealed<br />

slower response times to target items in comparison with control<br />

items when the intention was suspended but not when the intention<br />

was completed, thereby providing evidence for spontaneous<br />

retrieval only in the suspended condition. <strong>The</strong>se initial results suggest<br />

that spontaneous retrieval processes are quickly deactivated following<br />

completion of an intention.<br />

(1063)<br />

Attentional Manipulations Affect Focal and Nonfocal Prospective<br />

Memory Cues Differently. GABRIEL I. COOK, Claremont McKenna<br />

College, J. THADEUS MEEKS & RICHARD L. MARSH, University of<br />

Georgia, & GILLES O. EINSTEIN, Furman University—Event-based<br />

prospective memories involve retrieving intentions in the presence of<br />

an environmental cue. Such cues vary in their specificity, and consequently,<br />

the degree to which they evoke retrieval of the previously established<br />

intention. According to the multiprocess view of prospective<br />

memory, very specific cues (i.e., focal cues) are detected relatively automatically,<br />

whereas less specific cues (i.e., nonfocal cues) require<br />

more attentional resources to be devoted to intention completion to<br />

achieve the same level of performance. Three experiments used three<br />

different manipulations of reducing the attention available for intention<br />

completion in order to demonstrate that in all three cases the detection<br />

of nonfocal cues was more severely impacted than the detection<br />

of focal cues. <strong>The</strong> manipulations included level of effort required<br />

to perform the task in which cues were embedded, a response signal<br />

procedure, and a random number divided attention task. Thus, not all<br />

event-based cues require the same resources to be detected.<br />

(1064)<br />

Effects of Very Long-Term Penetrating Head Injury on Laboratory<br />

Prospective Memory. LIA KVAVILASHVILI, University of Hertfordshire,<br />

& VANESSA RAYMONT & JORDAN GRAFMAN, National<br />

Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke—Research on patients<br />

with traumatic brain injury (TBI) has shown that they experience<br />

deficits in remembering both event- and time-based prospective memory<br />

tasks (remembering to do something in response to an event or at<br />

a particular time in the future, respectively). However, very long-term<br />

effects of TBI on prospective memory have not been examined. <strong>The</strong><br />

present study assessed 199 Vietnam veterans with penetrating head injury<br />

(PHI) and 55 matched controls on three laboratory prospective<br />

memory tasks embedded within a general knowledge question answering<br />

task: a time-based task, and two event-based tasks with distinctive<br />

or nondistinctive target events. Performance of the control<br />

group on these tasks was not reliably better than that of the group with<br />

long-term PHI. Prospective memory performance will also be examined<br />

in relation to volume, hemisphere, and localization of the damage.<br />

(10<strong>65</strong>)<br />

Instructions to Complete or Forget an Intention Influence Stroop<br />

Performance. ANNA-LISA COHEN, Yeshiva University, &<br />

D. STEPHEN LINDSAY & JUSTIN KANTNER, University of Victoria—Research<br />

on the “intention superiority effect” (e.g., Goshke &<br />

Kuhl, 1993) revealed that material from scripts that are to be performed<br />

later by the participant are processed faster than is material<br />

60<br />

from a neutral script. Cohen, Dixon, and Lindsay (2005) demonstrated<br />

an “intention interference effect” for both young and older adults in<br />

which performance was slower for critical items belonging to an intention<br />

that participants intended to carry out versus critical items belonging<br />

to an intention that did not have to be executed. In a new line<br />

of studies, we examine the effect of critical item order. Because we<br />

used complex intentions that contained three critical items, we could<br />

examine how the superiority of the intention-related material builds<br />

across items. <strong>The</strong> results suggest that the first critical item acts as a<br />

reminder (showing no interference) and causes the remaining items to<br />

be retrieved reflexively, leading to Stroop interference.<br />

(1066)<br />

Effects of Delay and Type of Processing on Prospective Memory.<br />

KATHRYN A. HUDSON & DAWN M. MCBRIDE, Illinois State University—Prospective<br />

memory (PM) has been defined as the process<br />

of remembering future events. Multiprocess theory (McDaniel & Einstein,<br />

2000) proposes that PM performance can be more automatic<br />

when the PM and ongoing tasks are highly associated (i.e., focal PM<br />

cues are presented), but utilizes more controlled processes when the<br />

PM and ongoing tasks are not highly associated (i.e., nonfocal PM<br />

cues are presented). <strong>The</strong> present study examined how the delay between<br />

PM task instruction and PM cue presentation affects PM task performance<br />

with focal and nonfocal PM cues. As reported in previous studies<br />

(e.g., Einstein & McDaniel, 2005), responses to the ongoing task<br />

were faster in focal PM cue conditions than in nonfocal PM cue conditions.<br />

In addition, delay effects on PM task performance differed for<br />

focal and nonfocal conditions. <strong>The</strong> results of the present study support<br />

multiprocess theory descriptions of PM task performance.<br />

• RECALL •<br />

(1067)<br />

Recalling the List Before the Last: Context Isolation, Context Retrieval,<br />

and Filtering Retrieved Memories. YOONHEE JANG &<br />

DAVID E. HUBER, University of California, San Diego—A series of<br />

experiments used the list before the last free recall paradigm, which<br />

manipulated the list length of both target and intervening lists to index<br />

the degree of interference from each list. Correct target list recall was<br />

only affected by the target list length when participants engaged in recall<br />

between the lists, whereas there was an effect of both list lengths<br />

with other activities. This suggests that the act of recalling drives context<br />

change, isolating the target list from interference. Incorrect recall<br />

of intervening list items was affected only by the length of the intervening<br />

list when recall occurred between the lists, but was otherwise<br />

affected by both list lengths. A multinomial model of sampling and<br />

recovery was implemented to assess the adequacy of this account.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results suggest a more nuanced account of retroactive interference<br />

that includes context isolation, context retrieval, and the ability<br />

to filter retrieved memories.<br />

(1068)<br />

Memory by Sampling. CAROLINE MORIN & GORDON D. A.<br />

BROWN, University of Warwick—According to temporal distinctiveness<br />

models of memory such as SIMPLE (Brown, Neath, & Chater,<br />

2007), memory items’ temporal locations are represented as points on<br />

a temporal dimension in multidimensional psychological space. Such<br />

models do not provide a natural account of item presentation duration<br />

effects or rehearsal effects. Here, we explore predictions of a modified<br />

time-based model (memory by sampling) in which the temporal<br />

extension of items is explicitly represented. In Experiment 1, participants<br />

viewed lists of words whose presentation times followed either<br />

a U-shaped or an inverted U-shaped distribution. Participants overtly<br />

rehearsed each item during its presentation. In Experiment 2, presentation<br />

durations were random. Item recall increased systematically<br />

with item presentation time, especially in delayed free recall (Experiments<br />

3 and 4). <strong>The</strong> results were as predicted by the memory by sampling<br />

model.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!