S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

psychonomic.org
from psychonomic.org More from this publisher
29.01.2013 Views

Thursday Evening Posters 1049–1055 object-based ARE on a centrally located diamond-shaped object, consistent with the notion that the ARE originates early in perceptual processing. No object-based ARE was found with gaze cues. This suggests that these two forms of cues may have similar effects on temporal consequences of attention, but not on the spatial consequences. • METAMEMORY • (1049) Dissociating Judgments of Learning From Memory Encoding: An Event-Related Potential Study. IDA-MARIA SKAVHAUG, University of Stirling, EDWARD L. WILDING, Cardiff University, & DAVID I. DONALDSON, University of Stirling—Little is known about the neural basis for judgments of learning (JOLs). Here, we use event-related potentials (ERPs) to compare the neural correlates of JOLs and memory encoding. We ask whether JOLs are simply the result of memory encoding or are mediated by processes that are separate from (or additional to) encoding. In the study phase, participants assigned JOLs to a number of word pairs; memory for the pairs was assessed in a subsequent test phase. Trials were sorted on the basis of JOL and memory performance, allowing ERP activity recorded at study to be examined separately for encoding and JOL effects. An early positive-going posterior ERP effect (a typical subsequent memory effect) was associated with both memory encoding and JOLs, whereas a later negative-going left central ERP effect was present for JOLs only. The ERP findings dissociate the processes mediating JOLs from memory encoding. (1050) Learning About the Spacing Effect: The Role of Experience and Feedback. ALAN D. CASTEL, UCLA, & JESSICA M. LOGAN, Rice University—Previous research suggests that participants give higher judgments of learning (JOLs) for items practiced under massed rather than spaced conditions. We examined how practice with multiple testing sessions and feedback influenced participants’ JOLs for spaced and massed items. When participants made JOLs only after the second presentation of each item, JOLs did not differ for massed versus spaced items, despite memory performance favoring spaced rehearsal. When JOLs were made after each presentation and multiple study–test cycles were given, JOLs were strongly influenced by repetition, but did not differentiate between massed and spaced items. This occurred even when participants scored their own recall performance over multiple lists, suggesting that experience and self-directed feedback did not contribute to more accurate metacognitive judgments. Thus, although spaced rehearsal is a powerful learning technique, it appears that students underestimate the benefits of spaced repetition when learning. (1051) The Retrieval of Contextual Information Is Critical for Accurate Feeling of Knowing Predictions: An Aging Analysis. JOHN B. BULEVICH, Rhode Island College, AYANNA K. THOMAS, Tufts University, & STACEY J. DUBOIS, Colby College—In feeling of knowing (FOK) paradigms, participants are asked to predict subsequent recognition memory performance on items that were initially encoded, but that cannot presently be recalled. Research suggests that average FOK judgments may be influenced by the total amount of contextual information retrieved related to the unrecalled target (Koriat, 1993). We tested whether the episodic FOK deficit demonstrated by older adults relates to retrievability of partial contextual information. The effect of quantity and quality of partial information an average FOK judgments and prediction accuracy was compared in older and younger adults across three experiments. The results indicated that older adults required explicit retrieval of contextual information before making FOK judgments in order to make accurate FOK predictions. The results also demonstrated that quality of the retrieved partial information influenced both average FOK judgments and accuracy of FOK judgments in both older and younger adults. 58 (1052) Failure to Adjust Prediction of Knowing Judgments in a Memory Task Under Alcohol. MARK VAN SELST, JAYSON GAWTHORPE, & MARILYN AMPUERO, San Jose State University, & DANIEL CAMARILLO, University of Kentucky, Lexington—In an examination of the cognitive impact of acute alcohol intoxication, 16 participants were dosed in each of three alcohol conditions (0.00, 0.45, and 0.65 g/kg) counterbalanced across 3 days of testing. On each day, participants received the memory test at 65 min postdose (i.e., at or after peak BAC). During the study phase, the participants rated each word on how well they thought they would remember it (1 = not, 10 = definitely). Free recall of the word list was tested 5 min after the study phase; a recognition test was given immediately after the recall test. Performance on both tests declined with alcohol. Predictive memory ratings were not impacted by alcohol, although the predictive memory ratings remained as effective predictors of task performance in each of the alcohol conditions for both types of memory test. The paradoxical effect of work frequency was not strongly present in the recall data. (1053) Processes Underlying Metamemory Judgments in Patients With Schizophrenia. Investigation of the Accessibility Model. ELISABETH BACON, INSERM, & MARIE IZAUTE, Blaise Pascal University, Clermont-Ferrand—Cognitive deficits and insight problems are core symptoms of schizophrenia. The metamemory judgments of patients differ from the controls, whereas their predictive accuracy (G) is preserved (Bacon et al., 2001, 2007). According to Koriat (1993, 1995), the computation of FOK relies on the accessibility of partial and/or contextual information. Our aim was to see whether patients also rely on the products of retrieval to monitor their awareness about what they know and what they don’t know. The task was adapted from Koriat (1993). The material to be learned consisted of four-letter nonsense tetragrams, where each letter provided partial information with regard to the four-letter target. Patients showed global lower memory performances and metamemory ratings, but they showed remarkable consistencies between the retrieval of partial information and their metamemory ratings and accuracies. The accessibility hypothesis as a basis for metamemory judgments seems to be relatively preserved in patients with schizophrenia. (1054) Nicotine (2 mg), Attention, Memory, and Metacognition in Moderate Smokers. WILLIAM L. KELEMEN & ERIKA K. FULTON, California State University, Long Beach—Nicotine replacement therapies, including nicotine gum, can facilitate temporary abstinence in smokers, and thus support compliance with the increasing scope of smoke-free policies. This practice has been endorsed by the World Health Organization and adopted in businesses that require prolonged intervals of smoke-free activity, including some airlines and hospitals. We used a double-blind, within-subjects design to assess the cognitive effects (i.e., sustained attention, free recall, and metacognition) of 2 mg nicotine gum compared with a placebo (nicotine-free) gum. Moderate-to-heavy smokers were tested in both 8-h abstinent and nonabstinent states. They received one session of training followed by test sessions on consecutive days in four counterbalanced conditions: (1) abstinent with nicotine gum, (2) nonabstinent with nicotine gum, (3) abstinent with placebo gum, and (4) nonabstinent with placebo gum. Discussion will focus on methodological issues in nicotine research and whether nicotine produces cognitive benefits beyond relief of withdrawal in abstinent smokers. (1055) Adaptive Changes in Encoding Strategy With Experience: Evidence From the Test Expectancy Paradigm. JASON R. FINLEY & AARON S. BENJAMIN, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign—Efficient memory use requires that encoding decisions reflect future task demands. This experiment evaluated subjects’ abilities to adaptively

Posters 1056–1061 Thursday Evening modify encoding strategies. Across four study–test cycles, subjects were induced to expect either cued or free recall tests by studying lists of word pairs and receiving the same test format for each list. Tests required recall of target words, either in the presence (cued) or absence (free) of cue words. A fifth and final cycle included either the expected or the alternate, unexpected test format. On both cued and free final tests, subjects who had expected that format outperformed those who had not. Furthermore, cued-expecting subjects showed superior recognition of cue words and superior associative recognition of intact pairs. These results demonstrate that subjects were not merely modulating study effort based on anticipated test difficulty, but were adopting qualitatively different encoding strategies that were appropriate to the demands of the expected test. (1056) Effects of Schematic Knowledge on the Relationship Between Judgments of Learning (JOLs) and Memory for Items and Sources. AGNIESZKA E. KONOPKA & AARON S. BENJAMIN, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign (sponsored by Susan M. Garnsey)— Source monitoring judgments can reflect a combination of episodic recall and background knowledge. We investigated the extent to which metamnemonic judgments predict memory for items and memory for sources when schematic information is or is not provided at encoding. Participants gave judgments of learning (JOLs) to statements presented by two speakers, and were informed of the occupation of each speaker before or after the encoding session. Replicating earlier work, prior knowledge decreased the tendency to erroneously attribute statements to schematically consistent but episodically incorrect speakers. The origin of this effect can be understood by examining the relationship between JOLs and performance: In the absence of prior knowledge, JOLs were equally predictive of item and source memory, but were exclusively predictive of source memory when subjects knew of the relationship between speakers and statements during study. These results demonstrate meaningful influences of background knowledge on information that people solicit in service of metamnemonic judgments. (1057) Tip-of-the-Tongue States Interfere With Verbal Working Memory. BENNETT L. SCHWARTZ, Florida International University—Tipof-the-tongue states (TOTs) are judgments of the likelihood of retrieval for items currently not recalled. Schwartz (in press) showed that a concurrent verbal working memory task (digit span) lowered the number of TOTs relative to a control condition. In the present experiment, I examined whether unresolved TOTs led to lower performance in a verbal working memory task. First, participants answered general-information questions. Second, for unrecalled items, participants made TOT judgments. Then, on half of the unrecalled trials, participants were shown seven digits and were immediately asked to recall them. Participants were then given a recognition test for the unrecalled general-information questions. During TOTs, working memory performance was worse than it was for non-TOT items, demonstrating that interference occurs between TOTs and working memory. These behavioral data concur with neuroimaging data, which suggest that TOTs and verbal working memory activate similar areas in the brain (e.g., Maril et al., 2005). (1058) Is Testing Critical to Changing How One Learns? PATRICIA A. DEWINSTANLEY, Oberlin College, & ELIZABETH LIGON BJORK, UCLA—We examined whether a memory test is critical for changing the way that learners encode information during a second learning event. Participants studied paragraphs presented one sentence at a time on computer screens. During study of the first paragraph, some critical items had to be produced from letter fragments, whereas others were presented intact in bold letters. After the first paragraph, participants were tested on their memory for the information, were asked to predict which items they thought they would remember, or were 59 given an unrelated filler task. Next, all groups studied a second paragraph with both fragmented and intact critical items. Our research question asked under which conditions would participants change the way they processed the items during study of the second paragraph. We apply our findings to the illusion-of-comprehension literature and suggest ways that students might be able to improve their knowledge of their own learning. (1059) Illusions of Familiarity in Recognition Memory: The Role of Response Reversals. MARIANNE E. LLOYD, Seton Hall University, & JEREMY K. MILLER, Willamette University—Four experiments are presented that investigate whether participants are able to correct illusions of memory using the response reversal paradigm (Van Zandt & Maldonado-Molina, 2004), in which participants make both a speeded and a nonspeeded response to each recognition test item. The results of the study suggest that response reversals do not decrease rates of conjunction errors, even when participants employ encoding techniques that have been found to be effective in reducing conjunction errors (Lloyd, in press). In contrast, illusions due to artificial fluency (e.g., Jacoby & Whitehouse, 1989) can be reduced under limited circumstances. The results suggest that memory error rates and recognition sensitivity (which was improved for the second response in all conditions) are differently affected by opportunities to change one’s responses on a recognition test. • PROSPECTIVE MEMORY • (1060) Support for a Dual Process Model of Prospective Memory: Effects of Memory Load, Working Memory Capacity, and Asymmetrically Associated Lures. KEITH A. HUTCHISON, MARGARET T. FREUEN, & RYAN WORDEN, Montana State University (sponsored by Keith A. Hutchison)—Prospective memory (PM) performance was examined for target items (e.g., stork, string) using lexical decision as an ongoing task and varying memory load between 0, 2, and 6 items. In addition to PM target performance, responses were examined for critical forward-associated (e.g., baby) and backward-associated (e.g., puppet) lures in order to investigate presumed forward-acting active maintenance and backward-acting spontaneous retrieval processes, respectively. An n-back task was used to measure working memory capacity. Reaction times and errors for both PM targets and lexical decision filler words increased with memory load. As predicted by the dual process model, lure interference also increased with memory load, and this occurred exclusively for forward-related lures. Analysis of correlations further supported the dual process model in that backward interference correlated with PM performance only in the 2-item load, whereas forward interference and working memory capacity correlated with PM performance in the 6-item load. (1061) Reprioritizing Prospective Memory Intentions. ELAINE TAMEZ, SARAH C. MOYNAN, MARK A. MCDANIEL, & MEREDITH E. MINEAR, Washington University—Reprioritizing prospective memory (PM) intentions refers to a change in the order or the importance of an intention relative to an ongoing task or another intention. Marsh et al. (1998) found, in an observational study, that over half of incomplete intentions were due to reprioritizing intentions rather than forgetting intentions. The present experiments examine how reprioritizing PM intentions influences PM performance in a laboratorybased task. Two groups of participants were compared: those with a stable intention and those with an intention reprioritized relative to an ongoing task or to a second intention. PM performance declined relative to a stable intention group when the intention was reprioritized relative to an ongoing task but not when reprioritized relative to a second intention. Reprioritization also influenced reaction time on the ongoing task and strategy use. Our results indicate that reprioritization does not always negatively impact PM performance.

Thursday Evening Posters 1049–1055<br />

object-based ARE on a centrally located diamond-shaped object, consistent<br />

with the notion that the ARE originates early in perceptual processing.<br />

No object-based ARE was found with gaze cues. This suggests<br />

that these two forms of cues may have similar effects on temporal consequences<br />

of attention, but not on the spatial consequences.<br />

• METAMEMORY •<br />

(1049)<br />

Dissociating Judgments of Learning From Memory Encoding: An<br />

Event-Related Potential Study. IDA-MARIA SKAVHAUG, University<br />

of Stirling, EDWARD L. WILDING, Cardiff University, &<br />

DAVID I. DONALDSON, University of Stirling—Little is known<br />

about the neural basis for judgments of learning (JOLs). Here, we use<br />

event-related potentials (ERPs) to compare the neural correlates of<br />

JOLs and memory encoding. We ask whether JOLs are simply the result<br />

of memory encoding or are mediated by processes that are separate<br />

from (or additional to) encoding. In the study phase, participants<br />

assigned JOLs to a number of word pairs; memory for the pairs was<br />

assessed in a subsequent test phase. Trials were sorted on the basis of<br />

JOL and memory performance, allowing ERP activity recorded at<br />

study to be examined separately for encoding and JOL effects. An<br />

early positive-going posterior ERP effect (a typical subsequent memory<br />

effect) was associated with both memory encoding and JOLs,<br />

whereas a later negative-going left central ERP effect was present for<br />

JOLs only. <strong>The</strong> ERP findings dissociate the processes mediating JOLs<br />

from memory encoding.<br />

(1050)<br />

Learning About the Spacing Effect: <strong>The</strong> Role of Experience and<br />

Feedback. ALAN D. CASTEL, UCLA, & JESSICA M. LOGAN, Rice<br />

University—Previous research suggests that participants give higher<br />

judgments of learning (JOLs) for items practiced under massed rather<br />

than spaced conditions. We examined how practice with multiple testing<br />

sessions and feedback influenced participants’ JOLs for spaced<br />

and massed items. When participants made JOLs only after the second<br />

presentation of each item, JOLs did not differ for massed versus<br />

spaced items, despite memory performance favoring spaced rehearsal.<br />

When JOLs were made after each presentation and multiple study–test<br />

cycles were given, JOLs were strongly influenced by repetition, but<br />

did not differentiate between massed and spaced items. This occurred<br />

even when participants scored their own recall performance over multiple<br />

lists, suggesting that experience and self-directed feedback did<br />

not contribute to more accurate metacognitive judgments. Thus, although<br />

spaced rehearsal is a powerful learning technique, it appears<br />

that students underestimate the benefits of spaced repetition when<br />

learning.<br />

(1051)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Retrieval of Contextual Information Is Critical for Accurate<br />

Feeling of Knowing Predictions: An Aging Analysis. JOHN B.<br />

BULEVICH, Rhode Island College, AYANNA K. THOMAS, Tufts<br />

University, & STACEY J. DUBOIS, Colby College—In feeling of<br />

knowing (FOK) paradigms, participants are asked to predict subsequent<br />

recognition memory performance on items that were initially<br />

encoded, but that cannot presently be recalled. Research suggests that<br />

average FOK judgments may be influenced by the total amount of<br />

contextual information retrieved related to the unrecalled target (Koriat,<br />

1993). We tested whether the episodic FOK deficit demonstrated<br />

by older adults relates to retrievability of partial contextual information.<br />

<strong>The</strong> effect of quantity and quality of partial information an average<br />

FOK judgments and prediction accuracy was compared in older<br />

and younger adults across three experiments. <strong>The</strong> results indicated<br />

that older adults required explicit retrieval of contextual information<br />

before making FOK judgments in order to make accurate FOK predictions.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results also demonstrated that quality of the retrieved<br />

partial information influenced both average FOK judgments and accuracy<br />

of FOK judgments in both older and younger adults.<br />

58<br />

(1052)<br />

Failure to Adjust Prediction of Knowing Judgments in a Memory<br />

Task Under Alcohol. MARK VAN SELST, JAYSON GAWTHORPE,<br />

& MARILYN AMPUERO, San Jose State University, & DANIEL<br />

CAMARILLO, University of Kentucky, Lexington—In an examination<br />

of the cognitive impact of acute alcohol intoxication, 16 participants<br />

were dosed in each of three alcohol conditions (0.00, 0.45, and<br />

0.<strong>65</strong> g/kg) counterbalanced across 3 days of testing. On each day, participants<br />

received the memory test at <strong>65</strong> min postdose (i.e., at or after<br />

peak BAC). During the study phase, the participants rated each word<br />

on how well they thought they would remember it (1 = not, 10 = definitely).<br />

Free recall of the word list was tested 5 min after the study<br />

phase; a recognition test was given immediately after the recall test.<br />

Performance on both tests declined with alcohol. Predictive memory<br />

ratings were not impacted by alcohol, although the predictive memory<br />

ratings remained as effective predictors of task performance in<br />

each of the alcohol conditions for both types of memory test. <strong>The</strong><br />

paradoxical effect of work frequency was not strongly present in the<br />

recall data.<br />

(1053)<br />

Processes Underlying Metamemory Judgments in Patients With<br />

Schizophrenia. Investigation of the Accessibility Model. ELISABETH<br />

BACON, INSERM, & MARIE IZAUTE, Blaise Pascal University,<br />

Clermont-Ferrand—Cognitive deficits and insight problems are core<br />

symptoms of schizophrenia. <strong>The</strong> metamemory judgments of patients<br />

differ from the controls, whereas their predictive accuracy (G) is preserved<br />

(Bacon et al., 2001, 2007). According to Koriat (1993, 1995),<br />

the computation of FOK relies on the accessibility of partial and/or<br />

contextual information. Our aim was to see whether patients also rely<br />

on the products of retrieval to monitor their awareness about what they<br />

know and what they don’t know. <strong>The</strong> task was adapted from Koriat<br />

(1993). <strong>The</strong> material to be learned consisted of four-letter nonsense<br />

tetragrams, where each letter provided partial information with regard<br />

to the four-letter target. Patients showed global lower memory performances<br />

and metamemory ratings, but they showed remarkable consistencies<br />

between the retrieval of partial information and their<br />

metamemory ratings and accuracies. <strong>The</strong> accessibility hypothesis as<br />

a basis for metamemory judgments seems to be relatively preserved<br />

in patients with schizophrenia.<br />

(1054)<br />

Nicotine (2 mg), Attention, Memory, and Metacognition in Moderate<br />

Smokers. WILLIAM L. KELEMEN & ERIKA K. FULTON,<br />

California State University, Long Beach—Nicotine replacement therapies,<br />

including nicotine gum, can facilitate temporary abstinence in<br />

smokers, and thus support compliance with the increasing scope of<br />

smoke-free policies. This practice has been endorsed by the World<br />

Health Organization and adopted in businesses that require prolonged<br />

intervals of smoke-free activity, including some airlines and hospitals.<br />

We used a double-blind, within-subjects design to assess the cognitive<br />

effects (i.e., sustained attention, free recall, and metacognition)<br />

of 2 mg nicotine gum compared with a placebo (nicotine-free) gum.<br />

Moderate-to-heavy smokers were tested in both 8-h abstinent and nonabstinent<br />

states. <strong>The</strong>y received one session of training followed by test<br />

sessions on consecutive days in four counterbalanced conditions:<br />

(1) abstinent with nicotine gum, (2) nonabstinent with nicotine gum,<br />

(3) abstinent with placebo gum, and (4) nonabstinent with placebo<br />

gum. Discussion will focus on methodological issues in nicotine research<br />

and whether nicotine produces cognitive benefits beyond relief<br />

of withdrawal in abstinent smokers.<br />

(1055)<br />

Adaptive Changes in Encoding Strategy With Experience: Evidence<br />

From the Test Expectancy Paradigm. JASON R. FINLEY & AARON<br />

S. BENJAMIN, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign—Efficient<br />

memory use requires that encoding decisions reflect future task demands.<br />

This experiment evaluated subjects’ abilities to adaptively

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!