29.01.2013 Views

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

S1 (FriAM 1-65) - The Psychonomic Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Posters 1028–1034 Thursday Evening<br />

tion cost commonly attributed to task-set inhibition. An experiment is<br />

reported in which subjects were induced to chunk sequences such that<br />

n�2 repetitions occurred within or between chunks. Direct evidence of<br />

chunking was obtained and n�2 repetition cost was smaller when n�2<br />

repetitions occurred within chunks than between chunks. <strong>The</strong>se findings<br />

are consistent with the idea that the reduced n�2 repetition cost reflects<br />

priming of task goals rather than direct modulation of task-set inhibition.<br />

(1028)<br />

<strong>The</strong> Effect of Working Memory Load on Task Choice in Voluntary<br />

Task Switching. STARLA M. WEAVER & CATHERINE M. ARRING-<br />

TON, Lehigh University (sponsored by Catherine M. Arrington)—<strong>The</strong><br />

voluntary task switching (VTS) paradigm allows subjects to choose<br />

the specific task to be performed on each trial. Subjects are instructed<br />

to perform each task equally often and in a random order; however,<br />

they tend to show a repetition bias, performing fewer switches than<br />

would be expected in a random sequence. Lack of executive resources<br />

was assessed as a contributor to this repetition bias. A memory load<br />

manipulation was used to limit the executive resources available during<br />

VTS. Simple memory storage tasks, which required only the<br />

maintenance of memory letters, had no impact on VTS performance.<br />

However, a working memory load, which required the active manipulation<br />

of memory letters during VTS, differentially reduced the proportion<br />

of switches such that greater repetition bias was found under<br />

greater working memory loads. <strong>The</strong> availability of executive resources<br />

at the time of task performance appears to influence task choice.<br />

(1029)<br />

Directed Inhibition of Previewed Distractors in a Stroop Task:<br />

Underlying Mechanism. HSUAN-FU CHAO, Chung Yuan Christian<br />

University—In a previous study, it has been demonstrated that Stroop<br />

interference could be reduced by precuing the distracting word and<br />

asking participants to inhibit the previewed distractors in advance.<br />

This study aimed at investigating the underlying mechanism in this<br />

distractor previewing effect. Experiment 1 replicated the effect of distractor<br />

viewing when both incompatible and compatible trials were<br />

used. Experiments 2–4 demonstrated significant effects of distractor<br />

previewing when cues and distractors differed in forms. In Experiment<br />

5, a cost was obtained for conflict trials in that the previewed<br />

item cued the target color rather than the distractor word. Finally, the<br />

cue validity was removed in Experiment 6, and no effect of distractor<br />

previewing was observed. <strong>The</strong>se results ruled out the explanations in<br />

terms of response set, temporal segregation, and distractor facilitation.<br />

<strong>The</strong> intentional inhibition account can best explain these findings.<br />

(1030)<br />

What Happens to Attention When We Drink? GREGORY J. H.<br />

COLFLESH & JENNIFER WILEY, University of Illinois, Chicago—<br />

It is believed that alcohol use has a negative impact on attentional<br />

processes, but the affected cognitive mechanisms are not well understood.<br />

Clinical literature has advanced two theories describing how alcohol<br />

affects attention. According to the alcohol myopia theory, alcohol<br />

consumption decreases the focus of attention (Steele & Josephs,<br />

1990). Salience theories suggest that increased exposure to alcohol, or<br />

alcohol consumption, increases the salience of alcohol-related stimuli<br />

(Jones, Jones, Smith, & Copley, 2003), which acts as a source of distraction<br />

from other stimuli. <strong>The</strong> current study attempts to identify the<br />

attentional processes that alcohol use affects by examining performance<br />

on a battery of selective and divided attention tasks, including<br />

inattentional blindness, a modified Stroop task, and change blindness,<br />

while participants are intoxicated (BAC .075). Both neutral and alcoholrelated<br />

stimuli are used to test the myopia and salience accounts. Additionally,<br />

interactions between individual differences in WMC and<br />

changes in performance under intoxication will be discussed.<br />

(1031)<br />

Separating Exogenous From Endogenous Factors in Attentional<br />

Capture. MATTHEW YANKO, Simon Fraser University, PAOLA<br />

55<br />

POIESE, University of Trento, & THOMAS M. SPALEK & VINCENT<br />

DI LOLLO, Simon Fraser University—Attentional capture occurs<br />

when a task-irrelevant stimulus involuntarily receives attentional priority.<br />

Two sources of capture have been proposed in the literature: exogenous<br />

(stimulus-bound) and endogenous (goal-oriented). Although<br />

the two sources have often been regarded as mutually exclusive, we<br />

show that both can act concurrently and can be decoupled within a single<br />

experiment. <strong>The</strong> displays consisted of RSVP streams of differently<br />

colored letters with a target letter defined by a specific color. In some<br />

conditions, irrelevant color singletons surrounded either one RSVP<br />

item (Experiment 1) or all items (Experiment 2). Experiment 1 provided<br />

evidence for both exogenous and endogenous sources of capture.<br />

Experiment 2 reduced or eliminated exogenous sources by eliminating<br />

the uniqueness of the sudden onset of the peripheral<br />

distractors. <strong>The</strong> novel contribution of this work is that it provides unambiguous<br />

evidence that attention can be captured exogenously as<br />

well as endogenously. <strong>The</strong> results are consistent with dual-pathway accounts<br />

of attentional control.<br />

(1032)<br />

Decomposing the Multisource Interference Task. HEATHER M.<br />

GILMORE & ELISE TEMPLE, Cornell University (sponsored by<br />

Grover C. Gilmore)—<strong>The</strong> multisource interference task (MSIT) combines<br />

different types of interference known to delay reaction time for<br />

the purpose of maximizing cognitive interference. Previous work has<br />

not addressed the separate influences of each source of interference.<br />

<strong>The</strong> present study was designed to decompose the sources of interference<br />

to determine their independent contributions. Participants<br />

were instructed to identify the number that was different in a threedigit<br />

array. Experiment 1 examined flanker interference, the combination<br />

of flanker and spatial interference, and the effect of a font cue.<br />

Additionally, blocked versus mixed design was compared. It was<br />

found that two types of interference were more difficult than one type<br />

of interference and that blocking of interference type yielded more potent<br />

effects. Experiment 2 replicated the results and additionally examined<br />

spatial interference. <strong>The</strong> results of the study can be used to design<br />

a maximally potent MSIT.<br />

(1033)<br />

Goal Activation or Inhibition? Disentangling Forms of Top-Down<br />

Control. MARY K. ASKREN & CINDY LUSTIG, University of<br />

Michigan—<strong>The</strong> role of goal activation in conflict resolution is well<br />

accepted, but inhibition’s role is more controversial. We used a taskswitching<br />

procedure to disentangle the effects of demands on goal activation<br />

versus inhibition. Goal activation demands were measured by<br />

rule-incompatibility costs: Poorer performance on trials for which the<br />

two task rules led to opposite responses as compared with trials where<br />

both rules led to the same response. <strong>The</strong>se rule-incompatible trials required<br />

subjects to activate the correct goal in order to produce the correct<br />

response. <strong>The</strong> results (n = 48 young adults) indicate that rule incompatibility<br />

only led to slowing when it was confounded with<br />

bottom-up, Stroop-like conflict. This pattern suggests that even when<br />

participants have the correct goal activated, additional inhibitory<br />

processes are needed to suppress bottom-up, stimulus-driven conflict.<br />

A follow-up study examines which of these processes—goal activation<br />

or inhibition—shows larger differences between young and old<br />

adults.<br />

• AUTOMATIC PROCESSING •<br />

(1034)<br />

Affect Priming Versus Sequential Affect Judgment. DEAN G. PUR-<br />

CELL, Oakland University, & ALAN L. STEWART, Stevens Institute<br />

of Technology—Sequences of two faces displaying facial affect were<br />

presented under conditions of affect priming and under conditions of<br />

sequential affect judgment (SAJ). <strong>The</strong> priming task required the observer<br />

to judge the affect of only the second of the two faces. Affect<br />

priming was found as well as an anger inferiority effect (AIE). <strong>The</strong>

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!